Pragmatics and SLI: within-group variations in discourse behaviors.

Selected discourse behaviors of children with specific language impairment (SLI) presenting expressive (E:SLI) or combined expressive-receptive deficits (E-R:SLI) were compared to each other and to chronological age-mates and younger mean length of utterance (MLU)-matched children with normal-language skills. The two SLI subgroups varied from each other on specific measures of turn-taking and cohesion. These findings imply the need for future normative work with SLI subgroups differing in receptive skill, and indicate that, in the interim, pragmatic research with this population will need to consider potential effects of receptive language status when interpreting variations in outcomes for discourse-based variables.

[1]  Michael Halliday,et al.  Cohesion in English , 1976 .

[2]  B Z Liles,et al.  Story grammar ability in children with and without language disorder: story generation, story retelling, and story comprehension. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[3]  D. Aram,et al.  Patterns of Language Behavior in Children with Developmental Language Disorders , 1975 .

[4]  L. Bloom,et al.  Language development and language disorders , 1979 .

[5]  B. Brinton,et al.  Responses to requests for clarification in linguistically normal and language-impaired children. , 1986, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[6]  H. Craig,et al.  Children's turn-taking behaviors Social-linguistic interactions , 1986 .

[7]  L. Snyder Communicative And Cognitive Abilities And Disabilities In The Sensorimotor Period. , 1978 .

[8]  M L Rice,et al.  Social interactions of speech- and language-impaired children. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[9]  T. Klee,et al.  The relation between grammatical development and mean length of utterance in morphemes , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  E. Schegloff,et al.  A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation , 1974 .

[11]  D. Bishop,et al.  Comprehension problems in children with specific language impairment: literal and inferential meaning. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[12]  T. Gallagher,et al.  Gaze and proximity as turn regulators within three-party and two-party child conversations. , 1982, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[13]  R S Chapman,et al.  Communication breakdowns in normal and language learning-disabled children's conversation and narration. , 1988, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[14]  K. Chapman,et al.  The communicative functions of lexical usage by language impaired children , 1982, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[15]  A. Benton Developmental Aphasia and Brain Damage , 1964 .

[16]  Margaret Lahey,et al.  Language disorders and language development , 1988 .

[17]  Communication behaviors of infants with hearing loss and their hearing mothers. , 1993 .

[18]  A. van Kleeck,et al.  Discourse devices used by language disordered children: a preliminary investigation. , 1981, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[19]  Alan G. Kamhi,et al.  Syntactic and Semantic Aspects of the Utterances of Language-Impaired Children: The Same Can Be Less. , 1984 .

[20]  L. Leonard,et al.  Speech style modifications of language-impaired children. , 1981, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[21]  D. Bishop,et al.  Conversational characteristics of children with semantic-pragmatic disorder. II: What features lead to a judgement of inappropriacy? , 1989, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[22]  L. Bloom,et al.  Complex sentences: acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[23]  C. Prutting,et al.  Cohesion in the discourse of normal and head-injured adults. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[24]  Lois Bloom,et al.  Adult-child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  T. Gallagher,et al.  An Investigation of Pragmatic Connectives within Preschool Peer Interactions , 1987 .

[26]  H. Craig,et al.  Access behaviors of children with specific language impairment. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[27]  B. Liles Production and comprehension of narrative discourse in normal and language disordered children. , 1985, Journal of communication disorders.

[28]  R S Chapman,et al.  The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. , 1981, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[29]  B. Liles Episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children with and without language disorder. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[30]  Noam Chomsky Knowledge of language: its nature, origin, and use , 1988 .

[31]  P. Prinz An investigation of the comprehension and production of requests in normal and language-disordered children. , 1982, Journal of communication disorders.

[32]  H. Craig,et al.  Language sample collection and analysis: interview compared to freeplay assessment contexts. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[33]  M. J. Demetras,et al.  The identification of language impairment in the selection of specifically language-impaired subjects. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[34]  D. Bishop,et al.  SPECIFIC LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT AS A MATURATIONAL LAG: EVIDENCE FROM LONGITUDINAL DATA ON LANGUAGE AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT , 1987, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[35]  Asa Kasher,et al.  On the pragmatic modules: A lecture , 1991 .

[36]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Subgroups of developmental language impairment , 1980, Brain and Language.

[37]  D. Bishop,et al.  Conversational characteristics of children with semantic-pragmatic disorder. I: Exchange structure, turntaking, repairs and cohesion. , 1989, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[38]  Conversations with children who are language impaired: asking questions. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[39]  C. Garvey,et al.  Timing and turn taking in children's conversations 1 , 1981 .

[40]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[41]  The development of pragmatic connectives. 4- and 6-year-Old Comparisons , 1988 .

[42]  P Tallal,et al.  Selection of children with specific language deficits. , 1981, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[43]  B. Liles Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language-disordered children. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[44]  T. Gallagher,et al.  Conversational aspects of the speech of language-disordered children: revision behaviors. , 1978, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[45]  C Snow,et al.  Child language data exchange system , 1984, Journal of Child Language.