Strategies influence neural activity for feedback learning across child and adolescent development

Learning from feedback is an important aspect of executive functioning that shows profound improvements during childhood and adolescence. This is accompanied by neural changes in the feedback-learning network, which includes pre-supplementary motor area (pre- SMA)/anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), superior parietal cortex (SPC), and the basal ganglia. However, there can be considerable differences within age ranges in performance that are ascribed to differences in strategy use. This is problematic for traditional approaches of analyzing developmental data, in which age groups are assumed to be homogenous in strategy use. In this study, we used latent variable models to investigate if underlying strategy groups could be detected for a feedback-learning task and whether there were differences in neural activation patterns between strategies. In a sample of 268 participants between ages 8 to 25 years, we observed four underlying strategy groups, which were cut across age groups and varied in the optimality of executive functioning. These strategy groups also differed in neural activity during learning; especially the most optimal performing group showed more activity in DLPFC, SPC and pre-SMA/ACC compared to the other groups. However, age differences remained an important contributor to neural activation, even when correcting for strategy. These findings contribute to the debate of age versus performance predictors of neural development, and highlight the importance of studying individual differences in strategy use when studying development.

[1]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Brain–behavior correlation in children depends on the neurocognitive network , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[2]  K. Miller Executive functions. , 2005, Pediatric annals.

[3]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[4]  P C Molenaar,et al.  Finite mixture distribution models of simple discrimination learning , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[5]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  BrainWeb: Online Interface to a 3D MRI Simulated Brain Database , 1997 .

[6]  M. Raijmakers,et al.  Costs and benefits of automatization in category learning of ill-defined rules , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Better than Expected or as Bad as You Thought? The Neurocognitive Development of Probabilistic Feedback Processing , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Mark de Rooij,et al.  A Three-Year Longitudinal Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Performance Monitoring and Test-Retest Reliability from Childhood to Early Adulthood , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[10]  Jin Fan,et al.  Preparatory activity and connectivity in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex for cognitive control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[11]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[12]  Hans C. Jessen,et al.  Applied Logistic Regression Analysis , 1996 .

[13]  S. A. R. B. Rombouts,et al.  Separable neural mechanisms contribute to feedback processing in a rule-learning task , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Han L J van der Maas,et al.  Distinct discrimination learning strategies and their relation with spatial memory and attentional control in 4- to 14-year-olds. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Prediction of Individual Brain Maturity Using fMRI , 2010, Science.

[16]  Ingmar Visser,et al.  Seven things to remember about hidden Markov models: A tutorial on Markovian models for time series , 2011 .

[17]  M. Speekenbrink,et al.  depmixS4: An R Package for Hidden Markov Models , 2010 .

[18]  R. H. Myers Classical and modern regression with applications , 1986 .

[19]  Conor V. Dolan,et al.  Source (or Part of the following Source): Type Article Title Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis Author(s) Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis , 2022 .

[20]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[22]  Brenda R. J. Jansen,et al.  Developmental and gender related differences in response switches after nonrepresentative negative feedback. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[23]  Lawrence R. Rabiner,et al.  A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition , 1989, Proc. IEEE.

[24]  A. V. van Duijvenvoorde,et al.  Evaluating the Negative or Valuing the Positive? Neural Mechanisms Supporting Feedback-Based Learning across Development , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  G. Schwarz Estimating the Dimension of a Model , 1978 .

[26]  K. Walhovd,et al.  Performance monitoring in children and adolescents: A review of developmental changes in the error-related negativity and brain maturation , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Verena D. Schmittmann,et al.  Multiple learning modes in the development of performance on a rule-based category-learning task , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Performance Feedback Drives Caudate Activation in a Phonological Learning Task , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Increased Brain Activity in Frontal and Parietal Cortex Underlies the Development of Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity during Childhood , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  Roy E. Welsch,et al.  Detecting and Assessing Collinearity , 2005 .

[34]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Neural dynamics of error processing in medial frontal cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[35]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[36]  E. Crone,et al.  Training the developing brain: a neurocognitive perspective , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[37]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test , 2006, NeuroImage.

[38]  Brenda R. J. Jansen,et al.  Decomposing developmental differences in probabilistic feedback learning: A combined performance and heart-rate analysis , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[39]  Susan M. Ravizza,et al.  Shifting set about task switching: Behavioral and neural evidence for distinct forms of cognitive flexibility , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[41]  Maartje E. J. Raijmakers,et al.  The Neural Coding of Feedback Learning across Child and Adolescent Development , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Thomas Nyffeler,et al.  Inhibitory control of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the anti‐saccade paradigm − a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[43]  D. Lagnado,et al.  Models of probabilistic category learning in Parkinson's disease: Strategy use and the effects of L-dopa , 2010 .

[44]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Error Monitoring Using External Feedback: Specific Roles of the Habenular Complex, the Reward System, and the Cingulate Motor Area Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Raijmakers,et al.  Cognitive strategy use as an index of developmental differences in neural responses to feedback. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[46]  H.L.J. van der Maas,et al.  How to detect cognitive strategies: commentary on 'Differentiation and integration: guiding principles for analyzing cognitive change'. , 2008, Developmental science.

[47]  David A. Belsley,et al.  Regression Analysis and its Application: A Data-Oriented Approach.@@@Applied Linear Regression.@@@Regression Diagnostics: Identifying Influential Data and Sources of Collinearity , 1981 .

[48]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[49]  K. Zanolie,et al.  Neural mechanisms supporting flexible performance adjustment during development , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[50]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Errors without conflict: Implications for performance monitoring theories of anterior cingulate cortex , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  S. Strakowski,et al.  Selective role for striatal and prefrontal regions in processing first trial feedback during single-trial associative learning , 2012, Brain Research.

[52]  L. Keele,et al.  A General Approach to Causal Mediation Analysis , 2010, Psychological methods.

[53]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental differences in learning and error processing: evidence from ERPs. , 2009, Psychophysiology.