Association between intensity or accumulating pattern of physical activity and executive function in community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional study with compositional data analysis

Objective Previous studies have suggested a positive association between physical activity (PA) and executive function in older adults. However, they did not adequately consider the compositional nature of daily time use and accumulated PA patterns. Therefore, this study aimed to examine the association between intensity or accumulated PA patterns and executive functions (inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) in community-dwelling older adults, considering the interaction of daily time spent in PA, sedentary behavior (SB), and sleep. Method This cross-sectional study used baseline data from a randomized controlled trial on the effect of exercise on cognitive function conducted between 2021 and 2022. Data from 76 community-dwelling older adults were used in the analysis. The time spent in PA and SB was assessed using an accelerometer, and sleep duration was self-reported. The Stroop task (inhibitory control), N-back task (working memory), and task-switching task (cognitive flexibility) were conducted to evaluate the subcomponents of executive function. Considering various potential confounders, compositional multiple linear regression analysis and compositional isotemporal substitution were performed to examine the association of PA with executive function and to estimate predicted changes in executive function in response to the hypothetical time-reallocation of movement behaviors, respectively. Results A longer time spent in light-intensity PA (LPA), relative to remaining behaviors, was associated with better Stroop task performance. Moreover, this association was stronger in LPA lasting longer than 10 min than in sporadic LPA. Additionally, theoretical 30 min/day time reallocation from SB or sleep to LPA was associated with better Stroop task performance (corresponding to approximately a 5%−10% increase). On the other hand, no significant associations of time spent in moderate- to vigorous-intensity PA with any subcomponents of executive function were observed. Conclusion LPA was positively associated with inhibitory control, and this association was stronger in bouts of LPA than in sporadic LPA. Moreover, reducing the time spent in SB or sleep and increasing the time spent in LPA, especially long-bout LPA, could be important measures for managing inhibitory control in late life. Future large longitudinal and intervention studies are needed to confirm these associations and reveal the causality and underlying mechanisms.

[1]  J. Dorrian,et al.  How are combinations of physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep related to cognitive function in older adults? A systematic review , 2021, Experimental Gerontology.

[2]  J. Pantel,et al.  Deducing the Impact of Physical Activity, Sedentary Behavior, and Physical Performance on Cognitive Function in Healthy Older Adults , 2022, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience.

[3]  Kenji Tsunoda,et al.  Dose–response relationships of accelerometer‐measured sedentary behaviour and physical activity with non‐alcoholic fatty liver disease , 2021, Alimentary pharmacology & therapeutics.

[4]  L. Lechner,et al.  The longitudinal associations between change in physical activity and cognitive functioning in older adults with chronic illness (es) , 2021, BMC Geriatrics.

[5]  茂子 井上,et al.  加速度計で調査した農村部在住高齢者の身体活動:NEIGE study , 2021 .

[6]  Y. Otobe,et al.  The Influence of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Physical Activity and New Incidence of Frailty among Initially Non-Frail Older Adults in Japan: A Follow-Up Online Survey , 2021, The journal of nutrition, health & aging.

[7]  Sara B. Festini,et al.  Executive functions and neurocognitive aging , 2021, Handbook of the Psychology of Aging.

[8]  R. Ross,et al.  A systematic review of compositional data analysis studies examining associations between sleep, sedentary behaviour, and physical activity with health outcomes in adults. , 2020, Applied physiology, nutrition, and metabolism = Physiologie appliquee, nutrition et metabolisme.

[9]  Changwei Li,et al.  Sleep, Sedentary activity, physical activity, and cognitive function among older adults: The National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2011-2014. , 2020, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[10]  J. Etnier,et al.  Effects of Exercise Training Interventions on Executive Function in Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis , 2020, Sports Medicine.

[11]  N. Gothe Examining the effects of light versus moderate to vigorous physical activity on cognitive function in African American adults , 2020, Aging & mental health.

[12]  M. Gerber,et al.  Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.

[13]  S. Chastin,et al.  Sedentary behaviour is associated with depression symptoms: Compositional data analysis from a representative sample of 3233 US adults and older adults assessed with accelerometers. , 2020, Journal of affective disorders.

[14]  Deborah A. Lowe,et al.  Contribution of executive functioning to instrumental activities of daily living in older adults , 2020, Applied neuropsychology. Adult.

[15]  S. Chastin,et al.  Cox regression survival analysis with compositional covariates: Application to modelling mortality risk from 24-h physical activity patterns , 2019, Statistical methods in medical research.

[16]  J. Murabito,et al.  Accelerometer-determined physical activity and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults from two generations of the Framingham Heart Study , 2019, Alzheimer's & dementia.

[17]  Y. Oshima,et al.  Validity of estimating physical activity intensity using a triaxial accelerometer in healthy adults and older adults , 2019, BMJ Open Sport & Exercise Medicine.

[18]  T. Hung,et al.  How the 2018 US Physical Activity Guidelines are a Call to Promote and Better Understand Acute Physical Activity for Cognitive Function Gains , 2019, Sports Medicine.

[19]  T. Hung,et al.  Effects of Acute Exercise Duration on the Inhibition Aspect of Executive Function in Late Middle-Aged Adults , 2019, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[20]  Karel Hron,et al.  The compositional isotemporal substitution model: A method for estimating changes in a health outcome for reallocation of time between sleep, physical activity and sedentary behaviour , 2019, Statistical methods in medical research.

[21]  J. Gray,et al.  PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy , 2019, Behavior research methods.

[22]  Aimee J. Nelson,et al.  Exercise-Induced Neuroplasticity: A Mechanistic Model and Prospects for Promoting Plasticity , 2019, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[23]  T. Hortobágyi,et al.  Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis , 2019, PloS one.

[24]  Heinrich René Liesefeld,et al.  Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs(?) , 2018, Behavior Research Methods.

[25]  Karel Hron,et al.  Compositional data analysis for physical activity, sedentary time and sleep research , 2018, Statistical methods in medical research.

[26]  Joe R. Nocera,et al.  Community-Based Activity and Sedentary Patterns Are Associated With Cognitive Performance in Mobility-Limited Older Adults , 2018, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[27]  K. Khunti,et al.  Associations of Physical Behaviours and Behavioural Reallocations with Markers of Metabolic Health: A Compositional Data Analysis , 2018, International journal of environmental research and public health.

[28]  H. Shimada,et al.  Objectively measured physical activity and cognitive function in urban‐dwelling older adults , 2018, Geriatrics & gerontology international.

[29]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Exercise for cognitive brain health in aging: A systematic review for an evaluation of dose , 2018, Neurology Clinical Practice.

[30]  Jamie C. Peven,et al.  Associations Between Short and Long Bouts of Physical Activity with Executive Function in Older Adults , 2018 .

[31]  W. Kraus,et al.  Moderate‐to‐Vigorous Physical Activity and All‐Cause Mortality: Do Bouts Matter? , 2018, Journal of the American Heart Association.

[32]  B. Lawlor,et al.  The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review , 2017, Systematic Reviews.

[33]  Tim Olds,et al.  INTEGRATING SLEEP, SEDENTARY BEHAVIOUR, AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY RESEARCH IN THE EMERGING FIELD OF TIME-USE EPIDEMIOLOGY: DEFINITIONS, CONCEPTS, STATISTICAL METHODS, THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK, AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS , 2017 .

[34]  N. Owen,et al.  Comparability of activity monitors used in Asian and Western-country studies for assessing free-living sedentary behaviour , 2017, PloS one.

[35]  E. Wenger,et al.  Contribution of neuroinflammation and immunity to brain aging and the mitigating effects of physical and cognitive interventions , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[36]  Madhu N. Rao,et al.  Sleep and Cognitive Performance From Teens To Old Age: More Is Not Better , 2017, Sleep.

[37]  Diane K. Ehlers,et al.  Replacing sedentary time with sleep, light, or moderate-to-vigorous physical activity: effects on self-regulation and executive functioning , 2017, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[38]  David Scott,et al.  Light physical activity is positively associated with cognitive performance in older community dwelling adults. , 2016, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[39]  T. Liu-Ambrose,et al.  What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review , 2016, British Journal of Sports Medicine.

[40]  Yoshio Nakata,et al.  Accuracy of Wearable Devices for Estimating Total Energy Expenditure: Comparison With Metabolic Chamber and Doubly Labeled Water Method. , 2016, JAMA internal medicine.

[41]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  The association between aerobic fitness and cognitive function in older men mediated by frontal lateralization , 2016, NeuroImage.

[42]  D. Dijk,et al.  Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. , 2016, Sleep medicine.

[43]  Dawn A. Skelton,et al.  Combined Effects of Time Spent in Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviors and Sleep on Obesity and Cardio-Metabolic Health Markers: A Novel Compositional Data Analysis Approach , 2015, PloS one.

[44]  M. Okamoto,et al.  Long-Term Mild, rather than Intense, Exercise Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Greatly Changes the Transcriptomic Profile of the Hippocampus , 2015, PloS one.

[45]  S. Arnold,et al.  Exercise, Sedentary Pastimes, and Cognitive Performance in Healthy Older Adults , 2015, American journal of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias.

[46]  Javier Palarea-Albaladejo,et al.  zCompositions — R package for multivariate imputation of left-censored data under a compositional approach , 2015 .

[47]  Zude Zhu,et al.  Evidence for reduced efficiency and successful compensation in older adults during task switching , 2015, Cortex.

[48]  Chobok Kim,et al.  Reduced frontal cortex efficiency is associated with lower white matter integrity in aging. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: An fNIRS study , 2014, NeuroImage.

[50]  Dori E. Rosenberg,et al.  Barriers to and Facilitators of Physical Activity Program Use Among Older Adults , 2014, Clinical Medicine & Research.

[51]  Kirk I. Erickson,et al.  Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume , 2014, Neurobiology of Aging.

[52]  Somnath Chatterji,et al.  Associations between sleep duration, sleep quality, and cognitive test performance among older adults from six middle income countries: results from the Study on Global Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE). , 2014, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[53]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[54]  R. Wilson,et al.  The relationship between physical activity and cognition in older Latinos. , 2012, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[55]  Catrine Tudor-Locke,et al.  A Catalog of Rules, Variables, and Definitions Applied to Accelerometer Data in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2003–2006 , 2012, Preventing chronic disease.

[56]  L. Vaughan,et al.  Executive function in daily life: Age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[57]  E. McAuley,et al.  Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience Aging Neuroscience , 2022 .

[58]  Russell R. Pate,et al.  The Evolving Definition of "Sedentary" , 2008, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[59]  Carl W. Cotman,et al.  Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[60]  I-Min Lee,et al.  Physical activity and public health: updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. , 2007, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[61]  Hideaki Soya,et al.  BDNF induction with mild exercise in the rat hippocampus. , 2007, Biochemical and biophysical research communications.

[62]  Karen A Schutzer,et al.  Barriers and motivations to exercise in older adults. , 2004, Preventive medicine.

[63]  Pamela K. Smith,et al.  Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[64]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Color-Word Matching Stroop Task: Separating Interference and Response Conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.

[65]  K. Arbuthnott,et al.  Trail Making Test, Part B as a Measure of Executive Control: Validation Using a Set-Switching Paradigm , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[66]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[67]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[68]  N. Squires,et al.  Construct validity in the Trail Making Test: what makes Part B harder? , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[69]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.