Evidence of separable spatial representations in a virtual navigation task.

Three experiments investigated spatial orientation in a virtual navigation task. Subjects had to adjust a homing vector indicating their end position relative to the origin of the path. It was demonstrated that sparse visual flow was sufficient for accurate path integration. Moreover, subjects were found to prefer a distinct egocentric or allocentric reference frame to solve the task. "Turners" reacted as if they had taken on the new orientation during turns of the path by mentally rotating their sagittal axis (egocentric frame). "Nonturners," by contrast, tracked the new orientation without adopting it (allocentric frame). When instructed to use their nonpreferred reference frame, both groups displayed no decline in response accuracy relative to their preferred frame; even when presented with reaction formats based on either ego or allocentric coordinates, with format unpredictable on a trial, both groups responded highly accurately. These findings support the assumption of coexisting spatial representations during navigation.

[1]  William H. Warren,et al.  Chapter 8 – Self-Motion: Visual Perception and Visual Control , 1995 .

[2]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  D. Kimura,et al.  Sex differences in route-learning , 1993 .

[4]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[5]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Spatial Perspective in Descriptions , 1996 .

[6]  M. May,et al.  Raumorientierung in virtuellen Umgebungen , 1997 .

[7]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[8]  M. Denis,et al.  Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route directions in the city of Venice , 1999 .

[9]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Automatic Updating of Body-Centered Spatial Relationships , 1998 .

[10]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  The Head-neck sensory motor system , 1992 .

[11]  G A Goodrich,et al.  Gender differences in Piagetian visual-spatial representation of verticality and horizontality. , 1993, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[12]  Doris Höll,et al.  Is It Possible to Learn and Transfer Spatial Information from Virtual to Real Worlds? , 2003, Spatial Cognition.

[13]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Linda J. Anooshian,et al.  Diversity Within Spatial Cognition: Memory Processes Underlying Place Recognition , 1996 .

[16]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Einführung in die kognitive Neuropsychologie , 1991 .

[18]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Behavioral Experiments in Spatial Cognition Using Virtual Reality , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[20]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[22]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[23]  T. McNamara,et al.  Orientation and perspective dependence in route and survey learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  C. Lawton Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety , 1994 .

[27]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Turn Left at the Church, Or Three Miles North , 1986 .

[28]  Mary K. Kaiser,et al.  Perceived Orientation in Physical and Virtual Environments: Changes in Perceived Orientation as a Function of Idiothetic Information Available , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[29]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Componential modeling of alternative strategies for performing spatial tasks , 1984 .

[30]  A. Wunderlich,et al.  Brain activation during human navigation: gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Touretzky,et al.  Cognitive maps beyond the hippocampus , 1997, Hippocampus.

[32]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.

[33]  P Péruch,et al.  Homing in Virtual Environments: Effects of Field of View and Path Layout , 1997, Perception.

[34]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Using virtual environments to assess directional knowledge , 2004 .

[35]  J. Paillard Motor and representational framing of space , 1991 .

[36]  T. McNamara,et al.  Multiple views of spatial memory , 1997 .

[37]  Giti Javidi,et al.  Virtual Reality and Education , 1999 .

[38]  R Sekuler,et al.  Optic Flow Helps Humans Learn to Navigate through Synthetic Environments , 2000, Perception.

[39]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Virtual reality and education , 1992, [Proceedings] 1992 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[40]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[41]  Luigi Pizzamiglio,et al.  Evidence for Separate Allocentric and Egocentric Space Processing in Neglect Patients , 1998, Cortex.

[42]  Wilfried Brauer,et al.  Spatial Cognition II , 2000, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[43]  R. Roof,et al.  Neonatal exogenous testosterone modifies sex difference in radial arm and morris water maze performance in prepubescent and adult rats , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[44]  J. Rieser,et al.  Sensitivity to Perspective Structure While Walking without Vision , 1986, Perception.

[45]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by Path Integration , 1982 .

[46]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[47]  A. Redish Beyond the Cognitive Map: From Place Cells to Episodic Memory , 1999 .

[48]  D. Sengelaub,et al.  Prenatal Gonadal Steroids Affect Adult Spatial Behavior, CA1 and CA3 Pyramidal Cell Morphology in Rats , 1998, Hormones and Behavior.

[49]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Neural Correlates of Encoding Space from Route and Survey Perspectives , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  A. Tröster,et al.  Gender differences in geographical knowledge , 1987 .

[51]  F. Gaunet,et al.  Virtual environments as a promising tool for investigating human spatial cognition. , 1998 .

[52]  C. Lawton STRATEGIES FOR INDOOR WAYFINDING: THE ROLE OF ORIENTATION , 1996 .

[53]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Using Realistic Virtual Environments in the Study of Spatial Encoding , 2000, Spatial Cognition.

[54]  Wilfried Brauer,et al.  Spatial Cognition III , 2003, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[55]  P. Bloom,et al.  Language and space , 1998 .

[56]  E S Johnson,et al.  Developmental patterns of spatial ability: an early sex difference. , 1987, Child development.

[57]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Neural correlates of individual differences in spatial learning strategies. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[59]  M. Jeanne Sholl,et al.  The Role of a Self-Reference System in Spatial Navigation , 2001, COSIT.

[60]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[61]  S. Bricogne,et al.  Neural Correlates of Topographic Mental Exploration: The Impact of Route versus Survey Perspective Learning , 2000, NeuroImage.

[62]  Sholl Mj,et al.  The relation between horizontality and rod-and-frame and vestibular navigational performance. , 1989 .

[63]  A. Allport,et al.  Independent reference frames in human spatial memory: Body-centered and environmentcentered coding in near and far space , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[64]  Francesca Pazzaglia,et al.  Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals. , 2001 .

[65]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  The neural basis of ego- and allocentric reference frames in spatial navigation: Evidence from spatio-temporal coupled current density reconstruction , 2006, Brain Research.

[66]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  Die Tunnelaufgabe: Eine Methode zur Untersuchung kognitiver Teilprozesse räumlicher Orientierungsleistungen , 2001 .

[68]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation Specificity in Representations of Place , 1997 .

[69]  Martin Raubal,et al.  Comparing the Complexity of Wayfinding Tasks in Built Environments , 1998 .

[70]  K. Kerns,et al.  Sex differences in spatial ability in children , 1991, Behavior genetics.

[71]  M. Just,et al.  Cognitive coordinate systems: accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. , 1985, Psychological review.

[72]  G Debus,et al.  [The "tunnel task": a method for examination of cognitive processes in spatial orientation performance]. , 2001, Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie.

[73]  S. Huettel,et al.  Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[74]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  The Role of Geographical Slant in Virtual Environment Navigation , 2003, Spatial Cognition.

[75]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[76]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Different Patterns in Aiming Accuracy for Head-Movers and Non-Head Movers , 1992 .

[77]  W. Meck,et al.  The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[78]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[79]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  How Are the Locations of Objects in the Environment Represented in Memory? , 2003, Spatial Cognition.

[80]  D S Touretzky,et al.  Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space , 1996, Hippocampus.

[81]  Harry Heft The role of environmental features in route-learning: Two exploratory studies of way-finding , 1979 .

[82]  A Berthoz,et al.  Parietal and hippocampal contribution to topokinetic and topographic memory. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[83]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[84]  Patrick Péruch,et al.  Spatial Orientation in Virtual Environments: Background Considerations and Experiments , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[85]  L. Nadel,et al.  Human Place Learning in a Computer Generated Arena , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[86]  Donald G. Stein,et al.  Gender-specific impairment on Morris water maze task after entorhinal cortex lesion , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[87]  Walter J. Perrig,et al.  Sensory and conceptual representations in memory: Motor images that cannot be imaged , 1989 .

[88]  R. De Beni,et al.  Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals , 2001 .

[89]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[90]  M. Sholl,et al.  The relation between horizontality and rod-and-frame and vestibular navigational performance. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.