Decomposing task-switching costs with the diffusion model.

In four experiments, task-switching processes were investigated with variants of the alternating runs paradigm and the explicit cueing paradigm. The classical diffusion model for binary decisions (Ratcliff, 1978) was used to dissociate different components of task-switching costs. Findings can be reconciled with the view that task-switching processes take place in successive phases as postulated by multiple-components models of task switching (e.g., Mayr & Kliegl, 2003; Ruthruff, Remington, & Johnston, 2001). At an earlier phase, task-set reconfiguration (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) or cue-encoding (Schneider & Logan, 2005) takes place, at a later phase, the response is selected in accord with constraints set in the first phase. Inertia effects (Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994; Allport & Wylie, 2000) were shown to affect this later stage. Additionally, findings support the notion that response caution contributes to both global as well as to local switching costs when task switches are predictable.

[1]  B. Hommel,et al.  Semantic generalization of stimulus-task bindings , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  Eric-Jan Wagenmakers,et al.  An EZ-diffusion model for response time and accuracy , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  S. Brown,et al.  A critical test of the failure-to-engage theory of task switching , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  T. Woodward,et al.  The influence of cue-task association and location on switch cost and alternating-switch cost. , 2002, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[8]  Karl Christoph Klauer,et al.  Process components of the Implicit Association Test: a diffusion-model analysis. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Ulrich Mayr Towards Principles of Executive Control: How Mental Sets are Selected , 2003 .

[10]  Iring Koch,et al.  Sequential task predictability in task switching , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[12]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Anticipatory reconfiguration elicited by fully and partially informative cues that validly predict a switch in task , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Cerebral White Matter Integrity Mediates Adult Age Differences in Cognitive Performance , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Taking the brain serious: introduction to the special issue on integration in and across perception and action , 2007, Psychological research.

[15]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task switching: Negative priming and competitor priming , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  K Rayner,et al.  Reading without a fovea. , 1979, Science.

[19]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Advance task preparation reduces task error rate in the cuing task-switching paradigm , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[20]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Conflict monitoring: computational and empirical studies , 2004 .

[21]  U. Mayr,et al.  Is there an age deficit in the selection of mental sets? , 2001 .

[22]  Francis Tuerlinckx,et al.  Fitting the ratcliff diffusion model to experimental data , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  Andreas Voss,et al.  A fast numerical algorithm for the estimation of diffusion model parameters , 2008 .

[24]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[25]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[27]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[29]  A. Kolmogoroff Confidence Limits for an Unknown Distribution Function , 1941 .

[30]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. , 2008, Psychological review.

[31]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[32]  N. Yeung,et al.  Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[36]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[38]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[39]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.

[41]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  Andreas Voss,et al.  Fast-dm: A free program for efficient diffusion model analysis , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[43]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[44]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Selecting a response in task switching: testing a model of compound cue retrieval. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[46]  Pierre Jolicœur,et al.  Dual-task interference and visual encoding , 1999 .

[47]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[48]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  Francis Tuerlinckx,et al.  Diffusion model analysis with MATLAB: A DMAT primer , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[50]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Aging and individual differences in rapid two-choice decisions , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[51]  A Baddeley,et al.  Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  I Koch,et al.  Automatic and intentional activation of task sets. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  B. Hommel,et al.  The costs and benefits of cross-task priming , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[55]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.

[56]  I. Koch,et al.  Dissociating cue-related and task-related processes in task inhibition: evidence from using a 2:1 cue-to-task mapping. , 2008, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[57]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[58]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[59]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[60]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Response-based strengthening in task shifting: evidence from shift effects produced by errors. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  EZ does it! Extensions of the EZ-diffusion model , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[62]  A. Allport,et al.  Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[63]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Principles of Learning and Memory , 2003 .

[64]  G. Legge,et al.  Is word recognition different in central and peripheral vision? , 2003, Vision Research.

[65]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A retrieval theory of priming in memory. , 1988, Psychological review.

[66]  Aysegul Aydin,et al.  The Critical Test , 2012 .

[67]  N. Meiran,et al.  Is there a general task switching ability? , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[68]  Christoph Stahl,et al.  How to make your own response boxes: A step-by-step guide for the construction of reliable and inexpensive parallel-port response pads from computer mice , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[69]  A. Voss,et al.  Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: An empirical validation , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[70]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Interpreting instructional cues in task switching procedures: the role of mediator retrieval. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[71]  U. Mayr Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[72]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching , 2008, Psychological research.

[73]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[75]  E. Crook,et al.  Word Recognition , 2010 .

[76]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[77]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Phasic alertness and the residual task-switching cost. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[78]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing procedure: are there "true" task switch effects? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[79]  S. Sternberg Discovering mental processing stages: The method of additive factors. , 1998 .

[80]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Individual differences in components of reaction time distributions and their relations to working memory and intelligence. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[81]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[82]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[83]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[84]  D Gopher,et al.  Switching tasks and attention policies. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[85]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Comparing switch costs: alternating runs and explicit cuing. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[86]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[87]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[88]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[89]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  How to use the diffusion model: Parameter recovery of three methods: EZ, fast-dm, and DMAT , 2009 .