Auditory and visual orienting responses in listeners with and without hearing-impairment.

Head movements are intimately involved in sound localization and may provide information that could aid an impaired auditory system. Using an infrared camera system, head position and orientation was measured for 17 normal-hearing and 14 hearing-impaired listeners seated at the center of a ring of loudspeakers. Listeners were asked to orient their heads as quickly as was comfortable toward a sequence of visual targets, or were blindfolded and asked to orient toward a sequence of loudspeakers playing a short sentence. To attempt to elicit natural orienting responses, listeners were not asked to reorient their heads to the 0 degrees loudspeaker between trials. The results demonstrate that hearing-impairment is associated with several changes in orienting responses. Hearing-impaired listeners showed a larger difference in auditory versus visual fixation position and a substantial increase in initial and fixation latency for auditory targets. Peak velocity reached roughly 140 degrees/s in both groups, corresponding to a rate of change of approximately 1 micros of interaural time difference per millisecond of time. Most notably, hearing-impairment was associated with a large change in the complexity of the movement, changing from smooth sigmoidal trajectories to ones characterized by abruptly changing velocities, directional reversals, and frequent fixation angle corrections.

[1]  Warner Fite,et al.  From the Psychological Laboratory of the University of Chicago: The monaural localization of sound. , 1901 .

[2]  Warner Fite,et al.  Contributions from the Psychological Laboratory of the University of Chicago: Further observations on the monaural localization of sound. , 1901 .

[3]  H. Wallach,et al.  The role of head movements and vestibular and visual cues in sound localization. , 1940 .

[4]  de Ng Dick Bruijn A combinatorial problem , 1946 .

[5]  L. A. Jeffress,et al.  Localization of High‐Frequency Tones , 1957 .

[6]  W R Thurlow,et al.  Effect of induced head movements on localization of direction of sounds. , 1967, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  R. Lambert Dynamic theory of sound-source localization. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  F. Tonning Auditory localization and its clinical applications. , 1975, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[9]  G Lidén,et al.  Sound localization with phase audiometry. , 1976, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[10]  Wynn Vt Simple reaction time--evidence for two auditory pathways to the brain. , 1977 .

[11]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  The Relationship between Reaction Time and Intensity in Discrete Auditory Tasks , 1979, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  J Field,et al.  Newborn infants orient to sounds. , 1979, Child development.

[13]  F L Wightman,et al.  Interaural time discrimination ability of listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. , 1980, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[14]  Noble Wg Earmuffs, exploratory head movements, and horizontal and vertical sound localization. , 1981 .

[15]  A. Duquesnoy The intelligibility of sentences in quiet and in noise in aged listeners. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  U Rosenhall,et al.  The influence of hearing loss on directional hearing. , 1985, Scandinavian audiology.

[17]  A. Macleod,et al.  Quantifying the contribution of vision to speech perception in noise. , 1987, British journal of audiology.

[18]  R Plomp,et al.  The effect of speechreading on the speech-reception threshold of sentences in noise. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  D R Perrott,et al.  Changes in head position as a measure of auditory localization performance: auditory psychomotor coordination under monaural and binaural listening conditions. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  W. Noble,et al.  Effects on sound localization of configuration and type of hearing impairment. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  J H Fuller Comparison of horizontal head movements evoked by auditory and visual targets. , 1996, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[24]  W Noble,et al.  The contribution of head motion cues to localization of low-pass noise , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  S. Perrett,et al.  The effect of head rotations on vertical plane sound localization. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  D. Zambarbieri,et al.  Eye-head coordination toward auditory and visual targets in humans. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[27]  J. Koehnke,et al.  Effects of sensorineural hearing loss on interaural discrimination and virtual localization. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  W. Ehrenstein,et al.  Influence of head-to-trunk position on sound lateralization , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Comparison of relative and absolute sound localization ability in humans. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[30]  W. M. Rabinowitz,et al.  Auditory localization of nearby sources. II. Localization of a broadband source. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  F L Wightman,et al.  Resolution of front-back ambiguity in spatial hearing by listener and source movement. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  C. Lorenzi,et al.  Sound localization in noise in hearing-impaired listeners. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  P F Seitz,et al.  The use of visible speech cues for improving auditory detection of spoken sentences. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  T Ricketts,et al.  The Impact of Head Angle on Monaural and Binaural Performance with Directional and Omnidirectional Hearing Aids , 2000, Ear and hearing.

[35]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Sound localization with eccentric head position , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  K. Grant,et al.  The effect of speechreading on masked detection thresholds for filtered speech. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  L C Populin,et al.  Human Gaze Shifts to Acoustic and Visual Targets , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[38]  J. Fuller,et al.  Head movement propensity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  J. Schwartz,et al.  Seeing to hear better: evidence for early audio-visual interactions in speech identification , 2004, Cognition.

[40]  Jeffery A. Jones,et al.  Visual Prosody and Speech Intelligibility , 2004, Psychological science.

[41]  Michael C. Dorris,et al.  Combined eye-head gaze shifts to visual and auditory targets in humans , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Richard L. McKinley,et al.  The Impact of Hearing Protection on Sound Localization and Orienting Behavior , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[43]  Frederick J. Gallun,et al.  The advantage of knowing where to listen. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[44]  Daniel J Tollin,et al.  Sound-localization performance in the cat: the effect of restraining the head. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  L. Populin Monkey Sound Localization: Head-Restrained versus Head-Unrestrained Orienting , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Michael A Akeroyd,et al.  The detection of differences in the cues to distance by elderly hearing-impaired listeners. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[47]  Simon Carlile,et al.  Compression of auditory space during rapid head turns , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  W. O. Brimijoin,et al.  Patterned tone sequences reveal non-linear interactions in auditory spectrotemporal receptive fields in the inferior colliculus , 2010, Hearing Research.