Effects of mnemonic strategy manipulation in a Von Restorff paradigm.

Subjects were instructed to use either rote or elaborative strategies to memorize words in a Von Restorff paradigm. When instructed to use rote strategies, subjects displayed a higher Von Restorff effect and a lower recall performance than when instructed to use elaborative strategies. Furthermore the amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related brain potential predicted subsequent recall only when subjects used rote strategies. When subjects used elaborative strategies, the relationship between P300 amplitude and subsequent recall was not observed. These results confirm and expand, in a within-subjects design, the results reported by Karis, Fabiani and Donchin (1984) who capitalized on different strategies used by different subjects. These results also lend support to a 3-phase model of the von Restorff effect.

[1]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[2]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[3]  S. R. Schmidt Encoding and retrieval processes in the memory for conceptually distinctive events. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  LATENCY VARIABILITY IN THE COMPONENTS OF THE AUDITORY EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL IN DEMENTING ILLNESS , 1985 .

[5]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  David B. Mitchell,et al.  Specificity in Nonsemantic Orienting Tasks and Distinctive Memory Traces. , 1978 .

[7]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  E. Donchin,et al.  After a Rash Action: Latency and Amplitude of the P300 Following Fast Guesses , 2021, Neurophysiology and Psychophysiology.

[9]  H. V. Restorff Über die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im Spurenfeld , 1933 .

[10]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  Minicomputers in the signal-averaging laboratory. , 1975 .

[11]  E. Donchin Event-related Brain Potentials: A Tool in the Study of Human Information Processing , 1979 .

[12]  R. P. Fisher,et al.  Interaction between encoding distinctiveness and test conditions. , 1981 .

[13]  E. Donchin,et al.  “P300” and memory: Individual differences in the von Restorff effect , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Charles P. Bird The isolation effect as a function of unique processing orientation. , 1980 .

[15]  N. Squires,et al.  The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.

[16]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Perception and the Conditioned Reflex , 1965 .

[17]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .

[18]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  E Donchin,et al.  On how P300 amplitude varies with the utility of the eliciting stimuli. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  Eugene B. Zechmeister,et al.  Orthographic Distinctiveness as a Variable in Word Recognition. , 1972 .

[21]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  Definition, Identification, and Reliability of Measurement of the P300 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential , 1987 .

[23]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  P300 and long-term memory: latency predicts recognition performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[24]  E. Donchin,et al.  P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[25]  W. Wallace REVIEW OF THE HISTORICAL, EMPIRICAL, AND THEORETICAL STATUS OF THE VON RESTORFF PHENOMENON. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  A. Dickinson Contemporary Animal Learning Theory , 1981 .

[27]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[28]  R. R. Hunt,et al.  The role of nonsemantic information in memory: Orthographic distinctiveness effects on retention , 1980 .

[29]  E. Donchin,et al.  COGNITIVE PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY: THE ENDOGENOUS COMPONENTS OF THE ERP , 1978 .

[30]  R. Verleger Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  T W Picton,et al.  Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.

[32]  David B. Mitchell,et al.  Independent effects of semantic and nonsemantic distinctiveness , 1982 .

[33]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials during initial encoding and recognition memory of congruous and incongruous words , 1986 .

[34]  C. C. Wood,et al.  ERPs predictive of subsequent recall and recognition performance , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[35]  A. Baddeley The concept of working memory: A view of its current state and probable future development , 1981, Cognition.

[36]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[37]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the conceptual foundations of cognitive psychophysiology , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[38]  E Halgren,et al.  Cognitive evoked potentials as modulatory processes in human memory formation and retrieval. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[39]  Carla M. Bosco,et al.  Event-related brain potentials as indices of information extraction and response priming. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[40]  Walter Ritter,et al.  The relation of P3b to prior events and future behavior , 1984, Biological Psychology.