Impaired Value Learning for Faces in Preschoolers With Autism Spectrum Disorder.

OBJECTIVE One of the common findings in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is limited selective attention toward social objects, such as faces. Evidence from both human and nonhuman primate studies suggests that selection of objects for processing is guided by the appraisal of object values. We hypothesized that impairments in selective attention in ASD may reflect a disruption of a system supporting learning about object values in the social domain. METHOD We examined value learning in social (faces) and nonsocial (fractals) domains in preschoolers with ASD (n = 25) and typically developing (TD) controls (n = 28), using a novel value learning task implemented on a gaze-contingent eye-tracking platform consisting of value learning and a selective attention choice test. RESULTS Children with ASD performed more poorly than TD controls on the social value learning task, but both groups performed similarly on the nonsocial task. Within-group comparisons indicated that value learning in TD children was enhanced on the social compared to the nonsocial task, but no such enhancement was seen in children with ASD. Performance in the social and nonsocial conditions was correlated in the ASD but not in the TD group. CONCLUSION The study provides support for a domain-specific impairment in value learning for faces in ASD, and suggests that, in ASD, value learning in social and nonsocial domains may rely on a shared mechanism. These findings have implications both for models of selective social attention deficits in autism and for identification of novel treatment targets.

[1]  So Hyun Kim,et al.  ASD in Infants and Toddlers , 2014 .

[2]  K. Pierce,et al.  Eye Tracking Reveals Abnormal Visual Preference for Geometric Images as an Early Biomarker of an Autism Spectrum Disorder Subtype Associated With Increased Symptom Severity , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  J. C. Matter,et al.  Feedback-driven trial-by-trial learning in autism spectrum disorders. , 2015, The American journal of psychiatry.

[4]  Hyoung F. Kim,et al.  Distinct Basal Ganglia Circuits Controlling Behaviors Guided by Flexible and Stable Values , 2013, Neuron.

[5]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Gaze Response to Dyadic Bids at 2 Years Related to Outcomes at 3 Years in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Subtyping Analysis , 2013, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[6]  M. Tomasello Culture and Cognitive Development , 2000 .

[7]  K. Pierce,et al.  Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. , 2011, Archives of general psychiatry.

[8]  C. Norbury Sources of variation in developmental language disorders: evidence from eye-tracking studies of sentence production , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  S. Tipper,et al.  Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  Attention, Reward, and Information Seeking , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Shinya Yamamoto,et al.  Reward Value-Contingent Changes of Visual Responses in the Primate Caudate Tail Associated with a Visuomotor Skill , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Benjamin W. Tatler,et al.  Eye Movements from Laboratory to Life , 2014 .

[13]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Dissociating Motivation from Reward in Human Striatal Activity , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  A. Der-Avakian,et al.  The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits , 2012, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. , 2003, Child development.

[16]  E. Murray,et al.  The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in choices based on reward value and reward contingency. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  W. Schultz Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  A. Klin,et al.  Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion , 2009, Nature.

[19]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Looking But Not Seeing: Atypical Visual Scanning and Recognition of Faces in 2 and 4-Year-Old Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  G. Baranek Autism During Infancy: A Retrospective Video Analysis of Sensory-Motor and Social Behaviors at 9–12 Months of Age , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[21]  G. Dichter,et al.  Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[22]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  A. Wetherby,et al.  Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[24]  A. Klin,et al.  Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Basal ganglia orient eyes to reward. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  G. Dawson,et al.  Reduced engagement with social stimuli in 6-month-old infants with later autism spectrum disorder: a longitudinal prospective study of infants at high familial risk , 2016, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.

[27]  J. Brian,et al.  Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. , 2015, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[28]  Ilya E. Monosov,et al.  What and Where Information in the Caudate Tail Guides Saccades to Visual Objects , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Stephen V. Shepherd,et al.  Social status gates social attention in monkeys , 2006, Current Biology.

[30]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Combined Effects of Attention and Motivation on Visual Task Performance: Transient and Sustained Motivational Effects , 2008, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[31]  B. Scassellati,et al.  Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder , 2011, Brain Research.

[32]  L. Itti Author address: , 1999 .

[33]  Noah J. Sasson,et al.  Visual Attention to Competing Social and Object Images by Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Precursors to Social and Communication Difficulties in Infants At-Risk for Autism: Gaze Following and Attentional Engagement , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[35]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Reward predictions bias attentional selection , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  A. Klin,et al.  Attention to Eyes is Present But in Decline in 2–6 Month-Olds Later Diagnosed with Autism , 2013, Nature.

[38]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Hyoung F. Kim,et al.  Basal ganglia circuits for reward value-guided behavior. , 2014, Annual review of neuroscience.

[40]  Scott Draves,et al.  The Electric Sheep Screen-Saver: A Case Study in Aesthetic Evolution , 2005, EvoWorkshops.

[41]  B. Anderson The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection , 2016, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Speech Disturbs Face Scanning in 6-Month-Old Infants Who Develop Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  R. Saunders,et al.  Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral flexibility, emotion regulation and value updating , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  C. Lord,et al.  Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. , 2006, Archives of general psychiatry.

[46]  Mark H. Johnson Subcortical face processing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  Shaun J. Canavan,et al.  BP4D-Spontaneous: a high-resolution spontaneous 3D dynamic facial expression database , 2014, Image Vis. Comput..

[48]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Impaired Learning of Social Compared to Monetary Rewards in Autism , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[49]  Mark H. Johnson Autism: Demise of the Innate Social Orienting Hypothesis , 2014, Current Biology.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[52]  F. Shic,et al.  Decreased Spontaneous Attention to Social Scenes in 6-Month-Old Infants Later Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2013, Biological Psychiatry.

[53]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Examining the phenotypic heterogeneity of early autism spectrum disorder: subtypes and short-term outcomes. , 2016, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[54]  D. Barch,et al.  Goal representations and motivational drive in schizophrenia: the role of prefrontal-striatal interactions. , 2010, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[55]  F. Shic,et al.  Multilevel Differences in Spontaneous Social Attention in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder. , 2016, Child development.

[56]  R. Schultz,et al.  Reward system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[57]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses , 2009, Psychological science.

[58]  S. Bölte,et al.  Brief Report: Lack of Processing Bias for the Objects Other People Attend to in 3-Year-Olds with Autism , 2014, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[59]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. , 2012, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[60]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Functional territories in primate substantia nigra pars reticulata separately signaling stable and flexible values. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Tony Charman,et al.  What you see is what you get: contextual modulation of face scanning in typical and atypical development , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[62]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[63]  Pavel Korshunov,et al.  Crowdsourcing-based multimedia subjective evaluations: a case study on image recognizability and aesthetic appeal , 2013, CrowdMM '13.

[64]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Monkeys Pay Per View: Adaptive Valuation of Social Images by Rhesus Macaques , 2005, Current Biology.

[65]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  How motivation and reward learning modulate selective attention. , 2016, Progress in brain research.

[66]  C. Hofsten,et al.  How children with autism look at events , 2009 .

[67]  R. Schultz,et al.  The social motivation theory of autism , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.