Attention and target selection for smooth pursuit eye movements

Two rhesus monkeys were trained to track a small moving target in the presence of a moving distractor. The target and distractor were distinguished by their color. Smooth pursuit eye movements were quantified in terms of the latency of the eye movement and the open- loop eye acceleration profile. Smooth pursuit latencies for single targets were on the order of 100 msec. When the target was paired with a distractor moving in the same direction as the target, pursuit latencies decreased to roughly 85 msec. When the target was paired with a distractor moving in the opposite direction, pursuit latencies increased to roughly 150 msec. The motion of the distractor had no significant effect on the eye acceleration profile. Experiments were performed to dissociate visual search for the target from pursuit initiation by providing a spatial cue rather than the color cue. These experiments showed that visual search necessarily preceded pursuit initiation only when the distractor moved in the opposite direction relative to the target. In this case, visual search contributed about 25 msec to the overall latency of pursuit. Control experiments showed that the monkey need not attend to the distractor in order for it to influence the latency of pursuit. A network model was developed in which units that represent the motions of the target and distractor compete against one another. Attention serves to bias the outcome of this competition toward the direction of the selected target. The performance of this network exhibits a striking parallel to the effect of the distractor on smooth pursuit latency.

[1]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  R. Wurtz Visual receptive fields of striate cortex neurons in awake monkeys. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Frontal eye-field lesions in monkeys. , 1972, Bibliotheca ophthalmologica : supplementa ad ophthalmologica.

[5]  J. Cowan,et al.  Excitatory and inhibitory interactions in localized populations of model neurons. , 1972, Biophysical journal.

[6]  A. Treisman Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli , 1977 .

[7]  山崎 篤巳 Smooth Pursuit Eye Movementの定量的測定法 , 1977 .

[8]  P. Brodal,et al.  The corticopontine projection in the rhesus monkey. Origin and principles of organization. , 1978, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  D Marr,et al.  A computational theory of human stereo vision. , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[10]  P. Brodal,et al.  The pontocerebellar projection in the rhesus monkey: An experimental study with retrograde axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase , 1979, Neuroscience.

[11]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[12]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  F A Miles,et al.  Long-term adaptive changes in primate vestibuloocular reflex. I. Behavioral observations. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A. Gibson,et al.  Corticopontine visual projections in macaque monkeys , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  D. Zee,et al.  Effects of ablation of flocculus and paraflocculus of eye movements in primate. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[17]  P. Brodal,et al.  Further observations on the cerebellar projections from the pontine nuclei and the nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis in the rhesus monkey , 1982, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[22]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Glickstein,et al.  Corticopontine projection in the rat: The distribution of labelled cortical cells after large injections of horseradish peroxidase in the pontine nuclei , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[24]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex , 1985, Vision Research.

[25]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  F. Ottes,et al.  Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[27]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Hysteresis in the perception of motion direction as evidence for neural cooperativity , 1986, Nature.

[29]  R. Eckmiller,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1986, Progress in brain research.

[30]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  G. Phillips,et al.  Cooperative phenomena in the perception of motion direction. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  W. Newsome,et al.  Directional pursuit deficits following lesions of the foveal representation within the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Response properties of dorsolateral pontine units during smooth pursuit in the rhesus macaque. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  D. A. Suzuki,et al.  Smooth-pursuit eye movement deficits with chemical lesions in the dorsolateral pontine nucleus of the monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Jerome A. Feldman,et al.  Connectionist Models and Their Properties , 1982, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Pursuit and optokinetic deficits following chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Cooperative computation of stereo disparity , 1988 .

[40]  J. Voogd,et al.  The Topographical Organization of Climbing and Mossy Fiber Afferents in the Flocculus and the Ventral Paraflocculus in Rabbit, Cat and Monkey , 1989 .

[41]  R. Desimone,et al.  Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[42]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Visual responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar flocculus during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. I. Simple spikes. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  C. Bruce,et al.  Smooth-pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  F A Miles,et al.  Effects of stationary textured backgrounds on the initiation of pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[47]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  C D Salzman,et al.  Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. , 1994, Science.

[49]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  S. Yamane,et al.  Neural activity in cortical area MST of alert monkey during ocular following responses. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Initial tracking conditions modulate the gain of visuo-motor transmission for smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[52]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.