Physical modeling applies to physiology, too

A physical model was utilized to show that the neural system can memorize a target position and is able to cause motor and sensory events that move the arm to a target with more accuracy. However, this cannot indicate in which coordinates the necessary computations are carried out. Turning off the lights causes the error to increase which is accomplished by cutting off one feedback path. The geometrical properties of arm kinematics and the properties of the kinesthetic and visual sensorial systems should be better known before inferences about higher levels of processing can be drawn. An acceptable model for a physical system should be able to account for the observations in a wide range of situations and in a manner that does not depend on its own representation but only on that of the represented system. Coincidentally, this necessity is currently receiving a fair bit of attention in the robotics research, although, at least in the case of mechanical systems, the question was settled satisfactorily by the physicists and the mathematicians of the last century. Bruynincks (1991) has listed four types of invariance which are required to construct a theory with physical relevance. Although discussed in an engineering context, these invariances are also needed for physiological models. These are: invariance (1) to a change in reference frame, (2) to a change in physical units, (3) to a change in mathematical representation, and (4) to a change in arbitrary choices. It is unfortunate that the model proposed by Flanders et al. fails all four requirements. In other terms, one could construct uncountable variations of the proposed model that could equally explain the data. The question of reference frame invariance is of course of direct relevance to the subject matter since the essence of the proposed model is to deal with coordinate changes. In the study and in this commentary, the human arm is ∗in Behavioral Brain Sciences 1992 15(2):342–343, open peer commentary on Flanders, M., Tillery, S. I. H., Soechting, J. F. Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation. Behavioral Brain Sciences 1992 15(2):309–320. treated as three articulated rigid bodies: the torso used as the ground body, the arm and the forearm. Fig. 1 shows a cartoon version of the human arm kinematic arrangement in terms of two joints and three rigid links. For the sake of brevity, it will be discussed only intuitively. It can be readily observed that four coordinates are required to describe its position (and velocity). These coordinates can be chosen among many arbitrary possibilities. One fundamental reason from an engineering viewpoint, and most certainly, also from a physiological view point, for choosing one set rather than another is their respective computational advantages. From an engineering perspective, the reasons for making that choice are given by the computing hardware available to us. From a physiological perspective, the question cannot be so easily answered. The lack of detailed knowledge of how neural computations are carried out makes it difficult to speak of computational advantages other than in very general terms, if indeed it is a factor influencing Nature’s choice. The discussion about coordinates is important because it directly affects that of reference frames. What has just been discussed may be illustrated by an example. In the case of computer control, the set

[1]  J. Simpson,et al.  The accessory optic system of rabbit. II. Spatial organization of direction selectivity. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[3]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[4]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Kinematic strategies and sensorimotor transformations in the wiping movements of frogs. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  W. Graf,et al.  A quantitative analysis of the spatial organization of the vestibulo-ocular reflexes in lateral- and frontal-eyed animals—I. Orientation of semicircular canals and extraocular muscles , 1984, Neuroscience.

[6]  D. E. Whitney,et al.  The mathematics of coordinated control of prosthetic arms and manipulators. , 1972 .

[7]  D. Elliott,et al.  Manual localization of lateralized visual targets. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  W. Rymer,et al.  Characteristics of synergic relations during isometric contractions of human elbow muscles. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  R. A. Davidoff Handbook of the spinal cord , 1983 .

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[11]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[12]  R Caminiti,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Ewert Advances in vertebrate neuroethology , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  F. Lacquaniti Central representations of human limb movement as revealed by studies of drawing and handwriting , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  G. Poggio,et al.  Stereoscopic mechanisms in monkey visual cortex: binocular correlation and disparity selectivity , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  E. Thelen,et al.  The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Perceptual deficits and the activity of the color-opponent and broad-band pathways at isoluminance. , 1990, Science.

[19]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Sanes,et al.  Effects of perturbations on accuracy of arm movements , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  S. Amari,et al.  Sensori-motor transformations in the brain (with a critique of the tensor theory of cerebellum). , 1985, Journal of theoretical biology.

[23]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[24]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. I. Relations between single cell discharge and direction of movement , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979 .

[26]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Computational maps in the brain. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  R Linsker,et al.  Perceptual neural organization: some approaches based on network models and information theory. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  T. Sejnowski Computational models and the development of topographic projections , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[29]  S. Wise The primate premotor cortex: past, present, and preparatory. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Path constraints on point-to-point arm movements in three-dimensional space , 1986, Neuroscience.

[31]  Adi Ben-Israel,et al.  Generalized inverses: theory and applications , 1974 .

[32]  L E Mays,et al.  Signal transformations required for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  F. Plum Handbook of Physiology. , 1960 .

[35]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle or one of limb orientation? , 1982, Brain Research.

[36]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation , 1987, Neuroscience.

[37]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[38]  R. Eckmiller Advanced neural computers , 1990 .

[39]  R. Andersen,et al.  The role of the posterior parietal cortex in coordinate transformations for visual-motor integration. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[40]  D L Sparks,et al.  Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. , 1986, Physiological reviews.

[41]  J. Macpherson Strategies that simplify the control of quadrupedal stance. I. Forces at the ground. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  F. Zajac,et al.  Determining Muscle's Force and Action in Multi‐Articular Movement , 1989, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[43]  Bruno Siciliano,et al.  A closed-loop inverse kinematic scheme for on-line joint-based robot control , 1990, Robotica.

[44]  Stuart C. Shapiro,et al.  Encyclopedia of artificial intelligence, vols. 1 and 2 (2nd ed.) , 1992 .

[45]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[46]  E. Knudsen,et al.  Horizontal and vertical components of head movement are controlled by distinct neural circuits in the barn owl , 1990, Nature.

[47]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  A parametric analysis of human saccades in different experimental paradigms , 1987, Vision Research.

[48]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Foundations of cognitive neuroscience , 1993 .

[49]  C Ghez,et al.  Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[50]  J. Summers Approaches to the study of motor control and learning , 1992 .

[51]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .

[52]  G E Alexander,et al.  Movement-related neuronal activity selectively coding either direction or muscle pattern in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  P. Fitts,et al.  S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[54]  Eileen Kowler Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[55]  R. Andersen Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  1990 Lectures in Complex Systems , 1991 .

[57]  RP Dum,et al.  The origin of corticospinal projections from the premotor areas in the frontal lobe , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[58]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[59]  W. Ferrell,et al.  The effect of loading on position sense at the proximal interphalangeal joint of the human index finger. , 1989, The Journal of physiology.

[60]  R. Penrose A Generalized inverse for matrices , 1955 .

[61]  Z. Hasan,et al.  Timing and magnitude of electromyographic activity for two-joint arm movements in different directions. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  M. Hoy,et al.  The role of intersegmental dynamics during rapid limb oscillations. , 1986, Journal of biomechanics.

[63]  J. Perkell,et al.  Variability in production of the vowels /i/ and /a/. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[64]  C. Koch,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: from computational theory to neuronal mechanisms. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[65]  L. Buck Boundary distance effects on overshooting. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[66]  M. Arbib,et al.  Opposition Space as a Structuring Concept for the Analysis of Skilled Hand Movements , 1986 .

[67]  Bruce M. Carlson,et al.  Morphogenesis and Pattern Formation , 1981 .

[68]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[70]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Neural networks for control , 1990 .

[71]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[72]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  N. Draper,et al.  Applied Regression Analysis , 1966 .

[74]  A. Pellionisz,et al.  Brain modeling by tensor network theory and computer simulation. The cerebellum: Distributed processor for predictive coordination , 1979, Neuroscience.

[75]  R. Andersen,et al.  The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[76]  D. Margolin Cognitive neuropsychology in clinical practice , 1992 .

[77]  S. Gandevia Illusory movements produced by electrical stimulation of low-threshold muscle afferents from the hand. , 1985, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[78]  L. Yardley Contribution of somatosensory information to perception of the visual vertical with body tilt and rotating visual field , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[79]  H. Sakata,et al.  Spatial properties of visual fixation neurons in posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Modification of saccadic eye movements by GABA-related substances. II. Effects of muscimol in monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[81]  J F Soechting,et al.  Parcellation of sensorimotor transformations for arm movements , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[82]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[83]  V. Gurfinkel,et al.  Anticipatory neck muscle activity associated with rapid arm movements , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[84]  A. Pellionisz Tensorial aspects of the multidimensional massively parallel sensorimotor function of neuronal networks. , 1988, Progress in brain research.

[85]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[86]  F Lacquaniti,et al.  The control of limb geometry in cat posture. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[87]  S. Gandevia,et al.  The distribution of muscular weakness in upper motor neuron lesions affecting the arm. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[88]  C. Slack Some characteristics of the 'Range effect'. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[89]  P. C. Knapp,et al.  Lectures on the Diagnosis of Diseases of the Brain , 1885 .

[90]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[91]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[92]  W. Rymer,et al.  Joint position sense: the effects of muscle contraction. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[93]  W. T. Bourbon,et al.  On the Accuracy and Reliability of Predictions by Control-System Theory , 1990, Perceptual and motor skills.

[94]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: dynamic aspects in the primate motor cortex , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[95]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Theoretical requirements for the interpretation of signals of intramuscular tension. , 1982, Journal of theoretical biology.

[96]  James Trevelyan,et al.  Sensing and control for sheep shearing robots , 1989, IEEE Trans. Robotics Autom..

[97]  N Hogan,et al.  Planning and execution of multijoint movements. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[98]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. I. Temporal properties; comparison with area 7a. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[99]  Martha Flanders,et al.  Sensory control of target acquisition , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[100]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[101]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man , 1987 .

[102]  S. Wray Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control. , 1986, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[103]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Controlling multijoint motor behavior. , 1987, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[104]  J. Kelso,et al.  Exploring a vibratory systems analysis of human movement production. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[105]  R. Llinás,et al.  The Functional Organization of the Olivo‐Cerebellar System as Examined by Multiple Purkinje Cell Recordings , 1989, The European journal of neuroscience.

[106]  M A Arbib,et al.  Schemas for the temporal organization of behaviour. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[107]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Transformation from Head- to Shoulder-Centered Representation of Target Direction in Arm Movements , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[108]  Richard D. Jones,et al.  Impairment and recovery of ipsilateral sensory-motor function following unilateral cerebral infarction. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[109]  Jack Phillips,et al.  Freedom in machinery , 1984 .

[110]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[111]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Mechanical properties of muscles: Implications for motor control , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[112]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Different Patterns in Aiming Accuracy for Head-Movers and Non-Head Movers , 1992 .

[113]  P. Grobstein,et al.  Neuronal organization underlying visually elicited prey orienting in the frog—II. Anatomical studies on the laterality of central projections , 1987, Neuroscience.

[114]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[115]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Neural network model of visual cortex for determining surface curvature from images of shaded surfaces , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[116]  Z. Hasan,et al.  Initiation rules for planar, two-joint arm movements: agonist selection for movements throughout the work space. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[117]  Eric L. Schwartz,et al.  Computational Neuroscience , 1993, Neuromethods.

[118]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Sensorimotor transformation during eye movements to remembered visual targets , 1991, Vision Research.

[119]  W. Rymer,et al.  Strategies for muscle activation during isometric torque generation at the human elbow. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[120]  Woolsey,et al.  Cortical Sensory Organization , 1981, Cortical Sensory Organization.

[121]  M Kuperstein,et al.  Neural model of adaptive hand-eye coordination for single postures. , 1988, Science.

[122]  H. P. Bowditch,et al.  A Comparison of Sight and Touch , 1882, The Journal of physiology.

[123]  G. C. Joyce,et al.  The forces generated at the human elbow joint in response to imposed sinusoidal movements of the forearm , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[124]  A. Pellionisz,et al.  Tensorial approach to the geometry of brain function: Cerebellar coordination via a metric tensor , 1980, Neuroscience.

[125]  P. Grobstein The nervous system/behavior interface: Levels of organization and levels of approach , 1987, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[126]  A. Pellionisz,et al.  Space-time representation in the brain. The cerebellum as a predictive space-time metric tensor , 1982, Neuroscience.

[127]  G E Alexander,et al.  Neural representations of the target (goal) of visually guided arm movements in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[128]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[129]  J. Ewert,et al.  Visuomotor Coordination: Amphibians, Comparisons, Models, and Robots , 1989 .

[130]  C Ghez,et al.  Gradual specification of response amplitude in human tracking performance. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[131]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations , 1983 .

[132]  D. Humphrey,et al.  Motor control : concepts and issues , 1991 .

[133]  J. Kalaska,et al.  A comparison of movement direction-related versus load direction- related activity in primate motor cortex, using a two-dimensional reaching task , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[134]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[135]  J F Soechting,et al.  A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[136]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[137]  L. Kempe Handbook of Physiology. Section I. The Nervous System , 1982 .

[138]  J. Mcilwain Topography of eye-position sensitivity of saccades evoked electrically from the cat's superior colliculus , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[139]  E. B. Knauft,et al.  The accuracy of positioning reactions as a function of their direction and extent. , 1948, The American journal of psychology.

[140]  Masakazu Konishi Centrally synthesized maps of sensory space , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[141]  W. Chapple,et al.  Kinematic analysis of the defense response in crayfish. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[142]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[143]  G. Holmes DISTURBANCES OF VISUAL ORIENTATION , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[144]  C J Worringham,et al.  Operator orientation and compatibility in visual-motor task performance. , 1989, Ergonomics.

[145]  C. S. Harris Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. , 1965, Psychological review.

[146]  P. Grobstein,et al.  Neuronal organization underlying visually elicited prey orienting in the frog—III. Evidence for the existence of an uncrossed descending tectofugal pathway , 1987, Neuroscience.

[147]  J. Paillard,et al.  Visual motion cues in prismatic adaptation: evidence of two separate and additive processes. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[148]  P. Grobstein Between the retinotectal projection and directed movement: topography of a sensorimotor interface. , 1988, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[149]  P. Grobstein,et al.  Neuronal organization underlying visually elicited prey orienting in the frog—I. Effects of various unilateral lesions , 1987, Neuroscience.

[150]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  An algorithm for the generation of curvilinear wrist motion in an arbitrary plane in three-dimensional space , 1986, Neuroscience.

[151]  S. Gandevia,et al.  The distribution of muscle weakness in upper motoneuron lesions affecting the lower limb. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[152]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[153]  D. Pedoe A course of geometry for colleges and universities , 1972 .

[154]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Psychophysical determination of coordinate representation of human arm orientation , 1984, Neuroscience.

[155]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[156]  M. Kuperstein,et al.  An adaptive neural model for mapping invariant target position. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[157]  A. Pellionisz,et al.  Coordination: a vector-matrix description of transformations of overcomplete CNS coordinates and a tensorial solution using the Moore-Penrose generalized inverse. , 1984, Journal of theoretical biology.

[158]  Richard A. Johnson,et al.  Applied Multivariate Statistical Analysis , 1983 .

[159]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[160]  D. Holding Accuracy of delayed aiming responses , 1968 .

[161]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Preparation for movement: neural representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[162]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[163]  C. Hofsten,et al.  The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[164]  T. Flash,et al.  Moving gracefully: quantitative theories of motor coordination , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[165]  John M. Foley,et al.  Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues , 1972 .

[166]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. III. Memory-contingent visual and saccade responses. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[167]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[168]  E. Poulton Range Effects in Experiments on People , 1975 .