Unconscious cueing effects in saccadic eye movements – Facilitation and inhibition in temporal and nasal hemifield
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Correlates of Capture of Attention and Inhibition of Return across Stages of Visual Processing , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] E. Maylor,et al. Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] L Weiskrantz,et al. Pattern of neuronal activity associated with conscious and unconscious processing of visual signals. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[4] G. Woodman,et al. Dissociations Among Attention, Perception, and Awareness During Object-Substitution Masking , 2003, Psychological science.
[5] Neelam Naikar,et al. A new component of visual orienting: Implicit effects of peripheral information and subthreshold cues on covert attention , 1999 .
[6] Avishai Henik,et al. Attending to the thalamus: inhibition of return and nasal‐temporal asymmetry in the pulvinar , 2002, Neuroreport.
[7] Jan Theeuwes,et al. The Influence of Blind Distractors on Eye Movement Trajectories in Visual Hemifield Defects , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] V. Lamme,et al. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[9] Petroc Sumner,et al. Naso-temporal asymmetry for signals invisible to the retinotectal pathway. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] M. Fahle,et al. Naso-temporal asymmetry of visual perception and of the visual cortex , 1988, Vision Research.
[11] Geraint Rees,et al. Visual FMRI responses in human superior colliculus show a temporal-nasal asymmetry that is absent in lateral geniculate and visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] A Cohen,et al. Extrageniculate vision in hemianopic humans: saccade inhibition by signals in the blind field. , 1990, Science.
[13] C. Ávila. Facilitation and inhibition of visual orienting as a function of personality , 1995 .
[14] S. Levay,et al. The complete pattern of ocular dominance stripes in the striate cortex and visual field of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[15] S. Sherman. Visual Fields of Cats with Cortical and Tectal Lesions , 1974, Science.
[16] G. Stelmach,et al. Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .
[17] P. Sumner. Inhibition versus attentional momentum in cortical and collicular mechanisms of IOR , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[18] T. Ro. Unconscious vision in action , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[19] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .
[20] D. Munoz. Commentary: saccadic eye movements: overview of neural circuitry. , 2002, Progress in brain research.
[21] P. McCormick. Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] A. Cowey,et al. Retinal ganglion cells that project to the superior colliculus and pretectum in the macaque monkey , 1984, Neuroscience.
[23] L. Weiskrantz. Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .
[24] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .
[25] Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al. Masking Disrupts Reentrant Processing in Human Visual Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] Tony Ro,et al. Unconscious processing of orientation and color without primary visual cortex. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[27] R. Klein,et al. Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Raymond M Klein,et al. Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[29] Robert W. Kentridge,et al. Attention without awareness in blindsight , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[30] Michael I. Posner,et al. 14 Attention and the Control of Movements , 1980 .
[31] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[32] Tony Ro,et al. Extrageniculate mediation of unconscious vision in transcranial magnetic stimulation-induced blindsight. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[34] Avishai Henik,et al. Extrageniculate Contributions to Reflex Visual Orienting in Normal Humans: A Temporal Hemifield Advantage , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[35] P. Calabresi,et al. Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] R Fendrich,et al. Inhibitory tagging of locations in the blind field of hemianopic patients. , 1997, Consciousness and cognition.
[38] U. Ansorge. Asymmetric influences of temporally vs. nasally presented masked visual information: Evidence for collicular contributions to nonconscious priming effects , 2003, Brain and Cognition.
[39] C. Umilta,et al. Inhibition of return in newborns is temporo-nasal asymmetrical , 1995 .
[40] P. Sumner. Negative and positive masked-priming – implications for motor inhibition , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[41] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[42] D. Munoz,et al. On your mark, get set: Brainstem circuitry underlying saccadic initiation , 2000 .
[43] H. Deubel,et al. Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[44] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[45] J. Sprague,et al. Interaction of Cortex and Superior Colliculus in Mediation of Visually Guided Behavior in the Cat , 1966, Science.
[46] R. Rafal,et al. A temporal/nasal asymmetry for blindsight in a localisation task: evidence for extrageniculate mediation , 2002, Neuroreport.
[47] J. Theeuwes,et al. Grabbing attention without knowing: Automatic capture of attention by subliminal spatial cues , 2007 .
[48] M Eimer,et al. A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[49] D. Maurer,et al. The oculomotor distractor effect in normal and hemianopic vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[50] A. Cowey,et al. Nasal and temporal retinal ganglion cells projecting to the midbrain: Implications for “blindsight” , 1995, Neuroscience.
[51] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Using auditory and visual stimuli to investigate the behavioral and neuronal consequences of reflexive covert orienting. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[52] C. Kennard,et al. Distinct Cortical and Collicular Mechanisms of Inhibition of Return Revealed with S Cone Stimuli , 2004, Current Biology.
[53] Thom Carney,et al. Orientation discrimination as a function of stimulus eccentricity and size: Nasal/temporal retinal asymmetry , 1988, Vision Research.
[54] D. Vorberg,et al. The time course of response inhibition in masked priming , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[55] S. Shipp. The brain circuitry of attention , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.