Cognition and Categorization

ion of those listed by the students? In general, we found that the event name itself combined most readily with superordinate noun categories; thus, one gets dressed with clothes and needs various kitchen utensils to make breakfast. When such activities were analyzed into their script elements, the basic level appeared as the level of abstraction of objects necessary to script the events; e.g., in getting dressed, one puts on pants, sweater, and shoes, and in making breakfast, one cooks eggs in a frying pan. With respect to prototypes, it appears to be those category members judged the more prototypical that have attributes that enable them to fit into the typical and agreed upon script elements. We are presently collecting normative data on the intersection of common events, the objects associated with those events and the other sets of events associated with those objects.2 In addition, object names for eliciting events are varied in level of abstraction and in known prototypicality in given categories. Initial results show a similar pattern to that obtained in the earlier research in which it was found that the more typical members of superordinate categories could replace the superordinate in sentence frames generated by subjects told to "make up a sentence" that used the superordinate (Rosch, 1977). That is, the task of using a given concrete noun in a sentence appears to be an indirect method of eliciting a statement about the events in which objects play a part; that indirect method showed clearly that prototypical category members are those that can play the role in events expected of members of that category. The use of deviant forms of object names in narratives accounts for several recently explored effects in the psychological literature. Substituting object names at other than the basic level within scripts results in obviously deviant descriptions. Substitution of superordinates produces just those types of narrative that Bransford and Johnson (1973) have claimed are not comprehended; for example, " The procedure is actually quite simple. First you arrange things into different groups. Of course, one pile may be sufficient [p. 400]." It should be noted in the present context that what Bransford and Johnson call context cues are actually names of basic-level events (e.g., washing clothes) and that one function of hearing the event name is to enable the reader to translate the superordinate terms into basic-level objects and actions. Such a translation appears to be a necessary aspect of our ability to match linguistic descriptions to world knowledge in a way that produces the "click of comprehension." On the other hand, substitution of subordinate terms for basic-level object names in scripts gives the effect of satire or snobbery. For example, a review ( Garis, 1975) of a pretentious novel accused of actually being about nothing more than brand-name snobbery concludes, "And so, after putting away my 10

[1]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[2]  E. Rosch,et al.  Structural bases of typicality effects. , 1976 .

[3]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  P. L. Adams THE ORIGINS OF INTELLIGENCE IN CHILDREN , 1976 .

[5]  E. Rosch The nature of mental codes for color categories. , 1975 .

[6]  E. Rosch Cognitive reference points , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[9]  城所 良明,et al.  The Salk Institute for Biological Studies(話題) , 1975 .

[10]  D. Bobrow,et al.  Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science , 1975 .

[11]  W. R. Garner The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .

[12]  M. J. Peterson,et al.  Visual detection and visual imagery. , 1974, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[13]  K. Nelson Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. , 1974 .

[14]  P. G. Neumann An attribute frequency model for the abstraction of prototypes , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[16]  F. Moore Cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .

[17]  E. Rosch ON THE INTERNAL STRUCTURE OF PERCEPTUAL AND SEMANTIC CATEGORIES1 , 1973 .

[18]  John D. Bransford,et al.  Considerations of some problems of comprehension. , 1973 .

[19]  George Lakoff,et al.  Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts , 1973, J. Philos. Log..

[20]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Pattern recognition and categorization , 1972 .

[21]  William H. Geoghegan,et al.  Speculations on the growth of ethnobotanical nomenclature , 1972, Language in Society.

[22]  P. Kay Taxonomy and Semantic Contrast , 1971 .

[23]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[24]  M. Posner,et al.  Perceived distance and the classification of distorted patterns. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  J. Borges Other Inquisitions, 1937-1952 , 1964 .

[26]  L. Beach Cue probabilism and inference behavior. , 1964 .

[27]  L. Beach Recognition, assimilation, and identification of objects. , 1964 .