The origin of symbols in the brain

Deacon’s (1997) book is an interesting attempt to explain the critical aspects of the evolution of language as the learning of symbolic relationships. Deacon blurs the traditional distinction between syntax and semantics by arguing that the meaning of symbols is primarily determined via the combinatorial relations between symbols, and only secondarily via an indexical relation between a symbol and a referent (Deacon 1997, Ch. 3). However, this account of how acquisition of symbols involves multiple hierarchies of associative learning has proved rather difficult to understand (Hurford 1997), and even more difficult to incorporate into an explicit representational model. In this article, we want to use Deacon’s theory as a platform for a more elaborated and precise model of symbol learning.

[1]  P. Harris,et al.  The cognitive prerequisites for language: insights from iterated learning , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[2]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.

[3]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[4]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[5]  A. Clark,et al.  Trading spaces: Computation, representation, and the limits of uninformed learning , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  Catherine L. Harris,et al.  The human semantic potential: Spatial language and constrained connectionism , 1997 .

[7]  D. Dennett How to do Other Things with Words , 1997, Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement.

[8]  Julia Gray,et al.  A Model of the Limbic System and Basal Ganglia: Application to anxiety and schizophrenia , 1995 .

[9]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  A theory of emotion and consciousness, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. , 1995 .

[10]  Ferdinand de Saussure Course in General Linguistics , 1916 .

[11]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals. , 1998 .

[12]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[13]  J. Morén,et al.  Dynamics of a Classical Conditioning Model , 1998 .

[14]  P. Thomas Schoenemann Syntax as an Emergent Characteristic of the Evolution of Semantic Complexity , 1999 .

[15]  C. Lowe,et al.  Testing for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of language-trained chimpanzees. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[16]  Christian Balkenius,et al.  A Computational Model of Context Processing , 2000 .

[17]  V. Bruce,et al.  Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[19]  Michael Oliphant,et al.  The Learning Barrier: Moving from Innate to Learned Systems of Communication , 1999, Adapt. Behav..

[20]  D. Dennett Kinds of Minds: Towards an Understanding of Consciousness , 1996 .

[21]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells Is It Spatial Memory or a Memory Space? , 1999, Neuron.

[22]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[23]  Juan-Carlos Gómez Language and thought: Some thoughts about the evolution of LADS, with special reference to TOM and SAM , 1998 .

[24]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  A Connectionist Simulation of the Empirical Acquisition of Grammatical Relations , 1998, Hybrid Neural Systems.

[25]  Douglas L. T. Rohde,et al.  Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small? , 1999, Cognition.

[26]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Conditioning And Associative Learning , 1983 .

[27]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Is imitation learning the route to humanoid robots? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Dario Floreano,et al.  A Computational Model of Context Processing , 2000 .