Optimal Sensorimotor Control in Eye Movement Sequences

Fast and accurate motor behavior requires combining noisy and delayed sensory information with knowledge of self-generated body motion; much evidence indicates that humans do this in a near-optimal manner during arm movements. However, it is unclear whether this principle applies to eye movements. We measured the relative contributions of visual sensory feedback and the motor efference copy (and/or proprioceptive feedback) when humans perform two saccades in rapid succession, the first saccade to a visual target and the second to a memorized target. Unbeknownst to the subject, we introduced an artificial motor error by randomly “jumping” the visual target during the first saccade. The correction of the memory-guided saccade allowed us to measure the relative contributions of visual feedback and efferent copy (and/or proprioceptive feedback) to motor-plan updating. In a control experiment, we extinguished the target during the saccade rather than changing its location to measure the relative contribution of motor noise and target localization error to saccade variability without any visual feedback. The motor noise contribution increased with saccade amplitude, but remained <30% of the total variability. Subjects adjusted the gain of their visual feedback for different saccade amplitudes as a function of its reliability. Even during trials where subjects performed a corrective saccade to compensate for the target-jump, the correction by the visual feedback, while stronger, remained far below 100%. In all conditions, an optimal controller predicted the visual feedback gain well, suggesting that humans combine optimally their efferent copy and sensory feedback when performing eye movements.

[1]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  A Real-Time State Predictor in Motor Control: Study of Saccadic Eye Movements during Unseen Reaching Movements , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[4]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Neural Correlates of Internal-Model Loading , 2006, Current Biology.

[5]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Reference frames in saccadic targeting , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  R. Leigh,et al.  Cerebellar control of ocular gaze stability , 1980, Annals of neurology.

[8]  R. E. Kalman,et al.  New Results in Linear Filtering and Prediction Theory , 1961 .

[9]  R A Abrams,et al.  Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  L. Optican,et al.  Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Donald Morrill SACCADES , 2010, Fourth Genre: Explorations in Nonfiction.

[12]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Virtual lesions of the anterior intraparietal area disrupt goal-dependent on-line adjustments of grasp , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[14]  D. Robinson The mechanics of human saccadic eye movement , 1964, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  M. Kawato,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Examination of Two Modular Architectures for Switching Multiple Internal Models , 2022 .

[16]  P. Viviani,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to peripherally discriminated visual targets. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  D. Zee,et al.  Oculomotor function in the rhesus monkey after deafferentation of the extraocular muscles , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[19]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[20]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[21]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[22]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Oculocentric spatial representation in parietal cortex. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  D. Pélisson,et al.  On-line modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals , 2003, Neuroreport.

[24]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[25]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[26]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  A. Straube,et al.  Fixation errors and timing in sequences of memory-guided saccades , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Cortical control of saccades in man. , 1991, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[29]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The Sources of Variability in Saccadic Eye Movements , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Steinbach,et al.  On the role of extraretinal signals for saccade generation , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  K. Ren,et al.  Regulation of spinal neuropeptide genes in a rat model of peripheral inflammation and hyperalgesia. , 1995, Progress in brain research.

[32]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  The planning and control of reaching movements , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Cortical control of ocular saccades in humans: a model for motricity. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[34]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Saccadic Suppression , 2002, Science.

[35]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Optimal Sensorimotor Integration in Recurrent Cortical Networks: A Neural Implementation of Kalman Filters , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Robert L. White,et al.  Subthreshold microstimulation in frontal eye fields updates spatial memories , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  C. Bruce,et al.  Topography of projections to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[38]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[40]  R C Miall,et al.  System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  W. Becker The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Metrics. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[44]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex , 2006, Nature.

[45]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Error-correcting mechanisms in large saccades , 1978, Vision Research.

[47]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Vying for dominance: dynamic interactions control visual fixation and saccadic initiation in the superior colliculus. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[48]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  J W Gnadt,et al.  Analysis of the frequency response of the saccadic circuit: system behavior. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  D. Munoz,et al.  Competitive Integration of Visual and Preparatory Signals in the Superior Colliculus during Saccadic Programming , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.