Exploring Dominant Types of Explanations Built by General Chemistry Students

The central goal of our study was to explore the nature of the explanations generated by science and engineering majors with basic training in chemistry to account for the colligative properties of solutions. The work was motivated by our broader interest in the characterisation of the dominant types of explanations that science college students use to make sense of phenomena under conditions of limited time and limited explicit knowledge about a topic. Explanations were collected in written form using two different quizzes that students completed under time constraints at the end of a two‐semester general chemistry course. Our study revealed that students’ ability to generate causal/mechanical explanations depended on the nature of the task. In general, students were more inclined or able to generate mechanistic explanations to account for boiling‐point elevation and freezing‐point depression than to make sense of osmotic flow. The analysis of the types of causal explanations built by the study participants suggests that students may be biased towards some causal models or explanatory modes characterised as causal‐additive and causal‐static in our work. A large proportion of the students built non‐causal teleological explanations to account for osmotic flow. None of the participants in our study used a dynamic model of matter as the basis for their explanations of any of the relevant phenomena; the idea of an underlying random process that is taking place at all times giving rise to emergent properties and behaviours was completely absent from their intuitive reasoning under conditions of limited time and knowledge.

[1]  Leema K. Berland,et al.  Making sense of argumentation and explanation , 2009 .

[2]  Kathy Garvin-Doxas,et al.  Understanding randomness and its impact on student learning: lessons learned from building the Biology Concept Inventory (BCI). , 2008, CBE life sciences education.

[3]  Christine Chin,et al.  Learning in Science: A Comparison of Deep and Surface Approaches. , 2000 .

[4]  June Trop Zuckerman Problem Solver's Conceptions about Osmosis , 1994 .

[5]  Mitchel Resnick,et al.  Turtles, termites, and traffic jams - explorations in massively parallel microworlds , 1994 .

[6]  J. Shea National Science Education Standards , 1995 .

[7]  M. Resnick,et al.  Thinking in Levels: A Dynamic Systems Approach to Making Sense of the World , 1999 .

[8]  Pier Luigi Luisi,et al.  Emergence in Chemistry: Chemistry as the Embodiment of Emergence , 2002 .

[9]  U. Wilensky,et al.  Complex Systems in Education: Scientific and Educational Importance and Implications for the Learning Sciences , 2006 .

[10]  B. Andersson,et al.  The experiential gestalt of causation: a common core to pupils’ preconceptions in science , 1986 .

[11]  Eleanor Abrams,et al.  The how's and why's of biological change: How learners neglect physical mechanisms in their search for meaning , 2001 .

[12]  Jon Ogborn,et al.  A causal framework for analysing alternative conceptions , 1992 .

[13]  S. Carey,et al.  Functional explanation and the function of explanation , 2006, Cognition.

[14]  M. Engle Book Review: Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook (2nd Ed.) , 1999 .

[15]  William A. Sandoval,et al.  The Quality of Students' Use of Evidence in Written Scientific Explanations , 2005 .

[16]  T. Lombrozo The structure and function of explanations , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Eliciting Self-Explanations Improves Understanding , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[18]  Tina A. Grotzer Learning to Understand the Forms of Causality Implicit in Scientifically Accepted Explanations , 2003 .

[19]  Rosemary S. Russ,et al.  Recognizing mechanistic reasoning in student scientific inquiry: A framework for discourse analysis developed from philosophy of science , 2008 .

[20]  Earl Woodruff,et al.  Explanations from intra- and inter-group discourse: Students building knowledge in the science classroom , 1997 .

[21]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[22]  David N. Perkins,et al.  Dimensions of Causal Understanding: the Role of Complex Causal Models in Students' Understanding of Science , 2005 .

[23]  H. Wellman,et al.  Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. , 1998 .

[24]  Thomas W. Shiland Probing for Understanding. , 2002 .

[25]  Joan Solomon,et al.  Children's Explanations. , 1986 .

[26]  James D. Slotta,et al.  Helping Students Understand Challenging Topics in Science Through Ontology Training , 2006 .

[27]  H. Bartov,et al.  Teaching students to understand the advantages and disadvantages of teleological and anthropomorphic statements in biology , 1981 .

[28]  P. Klaczynski A dual-process model of adolescent development: implications for decision making, reasoning, and identity. , 2004, Advances in child development and behavior.

[29]  H. Bartov Can Students Be Taught to Distinguish between Teleological and Causal Explanations , 1978 .

[30]  R. Driver,et al.  Children's Ideas in Science , 1985 .

[31]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Commonsense Conceptions of Emergent Processes: Why Some Misconceptions Are Robust , 2005 .

[32]  Joseph Krajcik,et al.  Supporting Students' Construction of Scientific Explanations by Fading Scaffolds in Instructional Materials , 2006 .

[33]  David E. Penner,et al.  Explaining systems: Investigating middle school students' understanding of emergent phenomena , 2000 .

[34]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Models in explanations, Part 2: Whose voice? Whose ears? , 1998 .

[35]  D. Kelemen,et al.  The Human Function Compunction: Teleological explanation in adults , 2009, Cognition.

[36]  Matthew W. Lewis,et al.  Self-Explonations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[37]  Ruth Amir,et al.  High school students’ difficulties in understanding osmosis , 1987 .

[38]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Students' predictions about the sensory properties of chemical compounds: Additive versus emergent frameworks , 2008 .

[39]  J. Lagowski National Science Education Standards , 1995 .

[40]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Intentional Stance: Developmental and Neurocognitive Perspectives , 2002 .

[41]  Matthew B. Miles,et al.  Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook , 1994 .

[42]  D. Kelemen Function, goals and intention: children’s teleological reasoning about objects , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Laurence Viennot Reasoning in Physics: The Part of Common Sense , 2001 .

[44]  Keith S. Taber,et al.  Conceptual Resources for Learning Science: Issues of transience and grain‐size in cognition and cognitive structure , 2008 .

[45]  E. Jungwirth Do Students Accept Anthropomorphic and Teleological Formulations as Scientific Explanations , 1979 .

[46]  F. Keil,et al.  Explanation and understanding , 2015 .

[47]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Explanations and Teleology in Chemistry Education , 2007 .

[48]  U. Goswami,et al.  Cognitive development : the learning brain , 2008 .

[49]  Vicente Talanquer,et al.  On Cognitive Constraints and Learning Progressions: The case of “structure of matter” , 2009 .

[50]  Kathleen E. Metz Development of explanation: Incremental and fundamental change in children's physics knowledge , 1991 .

[51]  K. Taber,et al.  LEARNERS’ EXPLANATIONS FOR CHEMICAL PHENOMENA , 2000 .

[52]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Explanation in Scientists and Children , 1998, Minds and Machines.

[53]  David Hammer,et al.  Misconceptions or P-Prims: How May Alternative Perspectives of Cognitive Structure Influence Instructional Perceptions and Intentions , 1996 .

[54]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Commonsense Chemistry: A Model for Understanding Students' Alternative Conceptions , 2006 .