Somatosensory target information is used for reaching but not for saccadic eye movements.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Gunnar Blohm,et al. Reaching around obstacles accounts for uncertainty in coordinate transformations. , 2020, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] Yon Visell,et al. Proprioceptive Localization of the Fingers: Coarse, Biased, and Context-Sensitive , 2020, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.
[3] Hiroki Nakamoto,et al. Predictive eye movements when hitting a bouncing ball. , 2019, Journal of vision.
[4] Gerome A Manson,et al. Auditory cues for somatosensory targets invoke visuomotor transformations: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence , 2019, PloS one.
[5] Gerome A Manson,et al. Rapid online corrections for upper limb reaches to perturbed somatosensory targets: evidence for non-visual sensorimotor transformation processes , 2019, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Gunnar Blohm,et al. Neck muscle spindle noise biases reaches in a multisensory integration task. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] E. Brenner,et al. Gaze when reaching to grasp a glass. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[8] Gunnar Blohm,et al. Vibrotactile information improves proprioceptive reaching target localization , 2018, PloS one.
[9] Heiner Deubel,et al. Independent selection of eye and hand targets suggests effector-specific attentional mechanisms , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[10] R. Andersen,et al. Lateral intraparietal area (LIP) is largely effector-specific in free-choice decisions , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[11] M. Longo. Hand Posture Modulates Perceived Tactile Distance , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[12] Katja Fiehler,et al. Spatial specificity of tactile enhancement during reaching , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[13] Katja Fiehler,et al. Gaze-centered coding of proprioceptive reach targets after effector movement: Testing the impact of online information, time of movement, and target distance , 2017, PloS one.
[14] Brian C J Moore,et al. Blindness enhances auditory obstacle circumvention: Assessing echolocation, sensory substitution, and visual-based navigation , 2017, PloS one.
[15] Katja Fiehler,et al. Enhancement and Suppression of Tactile Signals During Reaching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Jing Chen,et al. Role of motor execution in the ocular tracking of self-generated movements. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] K. Fiehler,et al. Mixed body- and gaze-centered coding of proprioceptive reach targets after effector movement , 2016, Neuropsychologia.
[18] K. Fiehler,et al. Kinesthetic information facilitates saccades towards proprioceptive-tactile targets , 2016, Vision Research.
[19] J. Wann,et al. Eye and hand movement strategies in older adults during a complex reaching task , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Simon Grant,et al. Gaze–grasp coordination in obstacle avoidance: differences between binocular and monocular viewing , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Why do movements drift in the dark? Passive versus active mechanisms of error accumulation. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] P. Haggard,et al. Dynamic Tuning of Tactile Localization to Body Posture , 2015, Current Biology.
[23] Laurence Mouchnino,et al. Opposed optimal strategies of weighting somatosensory inputs for planning reaching movements toward visual and proprioceptive targets. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] Katja Fiehler,et al. Effector movement triggers gaze-dependent spatial coding of tactile and proprioceptive-tactile reach targets , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[25] Eli Brenner,et al. Proprioception Is Robust under External Forces , 2013, PloS one.
[26] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Waiting for a hand: saccadic reaction time increases in proportion to hand reaction time when reaching under a visuomotor reversal , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[27] Eric A. Yttri,et al. Lesions of cortical area LIP affect reach onset only when the reach is accompanied by a saccade, revealing an active eye–hand coordination circuit , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[28] Katja Fiehler,et al. Reach endpoint errors do not vary with movement path of the proprioceptive target. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[29] Christopher A. Buneo,et al. The Proprioceptive Map of the Arm Is Systematic and Stable, but Idiosyncratic , 2011, PloS one.
[30] Eli Brenner,et al. Reweighting visual cues by touch. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[31] Katja Fiehler,et al. Testing the limits of optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive information of path trajectory , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al. Keep your eyes on the ball: smooth pursuit eye movements enhance prediction of visual motion. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] P. Cavanagh,et al. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[34] B. Edin,et al. Human Muscle Spindles Act as Forward Sensory Models , 2010, Current Biology.
[35] Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al. Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion , 2010, EuroHaptics.
[36] Knut Drewing,et al. Optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive movement information for the perception of trajectory geometry , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] C. Galletti,et al. Contribution of visual and proprioceptive information to the precision of reaching movements , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] S. Gandevia,et al. The kinaesthetic senses , 2009, The Journal of physiology.
[39] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] B. Edin,et al. Discharges in Human Muscle Receptor Afferents during Block Grasping , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] R. Johansson,et al. Gaze behavior when reaching to remembered targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Changing Reference Frames during the Encoding of Tactile Events , 2008, Current Biology.
[43] J D Crawford,et al. Proprioceptive guidance of saccades in eye-hand coordination. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[44] J. Wann,et al. How active gaze informs the hand in sequential pointing movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] R. Shadmehr,et al. Why Does the Brain Predict Sensory Consequences of Oculomotor Commands? Optimal Integration of the Predicted and the Actual Sensory Feedback , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[46] T. Vilis,et al. Directional selectivity of BOLD activity in human posterior parietal cortex for memory-guided double-step saccades. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] S. Gandevia,et al. Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[49] H Bekkering,et al. Coordinated control of eye and hand movements in dynamic reaching. , 2002, Human movement science.
[50] Chris Rorden,et al. Enhanced Tactile Performance at the Destination of an Upcoming Saccade , 2002, Current Biology.
[51] Stefan Everling,et al. Hand-eye coordination for rapid pointing movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] D. Wolpert,et al. When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.
[53] Christopher A. Buneo,et al. Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching , 2002, Nature.
[54] A. Pouget,et al. Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching , 2002, Cognition.
[55] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[56] O. Blanke,et al. Saccades guided by somatosensory stimuli , 2001, Vision Research.
[57] A. Gordon,et al. Contribution of tactile information to accuracy in pointing movements , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[58] H. Bekkering,et al. Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[59] R. J. van Beers,et al. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[60] M. Land,et al. The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.
[61] Jean-Louis Vercher,et al. Manuo-ocular coordination in target tracking. I. A model simulating human performance , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.
[62] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.
[63] Yiannis Aloimonos,et al. Vision and action , 1995, Image Vis. Comput..
[64] B. Edin,et al. Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.
[65] C. Prablanc,et al. Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[66] J. F. Soechting,et al. Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[67] Otmar Bock,et al. Coordination of arm and eye movements in tracking of sinusoidally moving targets , 1987, Behavioural Brain Research.
[68] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[69] Katja Fiehler,et al. Saccades and reaches curve away from the other effector's target in simultaneous eye and hand movements. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.
[70] Jos J. Adam,et al. Reaction time latencies of eye and hand movements in single- and dual-task conditions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[71] C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al. The attainment of target position during step-tracking movements despite a shift of initial position , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[72] Mary M Hayhoe,et al. Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[73] D. Sparks,et al. Saccades to somatosensory targets. I. behavioral characteristics. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[74] J. Gordon,et al. Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.