Selective Attention to Specific Features within Objects: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence

Evidence regarding the ability of attention to bias neural processing at the level of single features has been gathering steadily, but most of the experiments to date used arrays with multiple objects and locations, making it difficult to rule out indirect influences from object or spatial attention. To investigate feature-specific selective attention, we have assessed the ability to select and ignore individual features within the same object. We used a negative-priming paradigm in which the color or the direction of internal motion of the object could determine the relevant response. Bidimensional (colored and moving) and unidimensional (colored and stationary, or gray and moving) stimuli appeared in unpredictable order. In successive blocks, participants were instructed that one feature dimension was dominant. During that block, participants responded according to the dominant dimension for bidimensional stimuli. For unidimensional stimuli, participants responded to the only dimension of the stimulus that afforded a response, regardless of the instruction for the block. The ability to inhibit irrelevant task information at the level of specific features (negative priming for features) was indexed by a decrease in performance to detect one particular feature value (e.g., red) if the same feature value (red) but not another color value (green) had been ignored in the previous bidimensional stimulus. Behavioral results confirmed the existence of inhibitory, negative-priming mechanisms at the singlefeature level for both color and motion dimensions of stimuli. Event-related potentials recorded during task performance revealed the dynamics of neural modulation by feature attention. Comparisons were made using the identical physical stimuli under different conditions of attention to isolate purely attentional effects. Processing of identical bidimensional stimuli was compared as a function of the dimension of attention (color, motion). Processing of identical unidimensional stimuli that followed bidimensional stimuli was also compared to identify possible effects of feature-specific negative priming. The electrophysiological effects revealed that inhibition of irrelevant features leads to modulation of brain activity during early stages of perceptual analysis.

[1]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.

[2]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  J Duncan,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  A. Nobre,et al.  The dynamics of shifting visuospatial attention revealed by event-related potentials , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Neurons in Area V4 of the Macaque Translate Attended Visual Features into Behaviorally Relevant Categories , 2007, Neuron.

[6]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  M. Posner The Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention , 2020 .

[9]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Selective attention to the color and direction of moving stimuli: Electrophysiological correlates of hierarchical feature selection , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  R. Passingham,et al.  The left parietal cortex and motor attention , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[12]  A. Treisman,et al.  Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Anna Christina Nobre Probing the Flexibility of Attentional Orienting in the Human Brain. , 2004 .

[14]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[15]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[16]  E. Halgren,et al.  The intracranial topography of the P3 event-related potential elicited during auditory oddball. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  P. Brodal The Central Nervous System , 1992 .

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[20]  A E Seiffert,et al.  Selective attention: a reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. , 1998, Psychological review.

[21]  Valia Rodríguez,et al.  Dividing attention between form and motion during transparent surface perception. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  A. Nobre The attentive homunculus: Now you see it, now you don't , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[24]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  H. Müller,et al.  Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Evidence for parallel-coactive processing of dimensions , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[27]  J. Duncan,et al.  Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  S. Yamaguchi,et al.  Anterior and posterior association cortex contributions to the somatosensory P300 , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[30]  B Giesbrecht,et al.  Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[31]  M. Eimer An event-related potential (ERP) study of transient and sustained visual attention to color and form , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[32]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Spatial Repetition Blindness Is Modulated by Selective Attention to Color or Shape , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  R. M. Boynton Human color vision , 1979 .

[34]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[35]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[36]  W. Neill,et al.  Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention. , 1977 .

[37]  A C Nobre,et al.  Components of attentional set-switching. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[38]  J. Polich P300 clinical utility and control of variability. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[39]  E. Donchin,et al.  Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .

[40]  S. Bisti,et al.  Light sensitivity, adaptation and saturation in mammalian rods. , 1993, Progress in brain research.

[41]  M. Valdés-Sosa,et al.  Transparent motion and object-based attention , 1998, Cognition.

[42]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Frontal processing negativity in a visual selective attention task. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  S. Luck,et al.  Attention to Features Precedes Attention to Locations in Visual Search: Evidence from Electromagnetic Brain Responses in Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  S. Treue,et al.  Feature-Based Attention Increases the Selectivity of Population Responses in Primate Visual Cortex , 2004, Current Biology.

[45]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[46]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[47]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time. Modulation of brain potentials. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  E Donchin,et al.  A cortical potential imaging analysis of the P300 and Novelty P3 components , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[49]  N Kanwisher,et al.  Negative priming for spatial locations: identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  M. Paradiso,et al.  Feature-specific effects of selective visual attention , 1995, Vision Research.

[51]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[52]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Selecting and ignoring the component features of a visual object: A negative priming paradigm , 2006 .

[53]  C C Wood,et al.  Letter: The epsilon-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[54]  G. Boynton,et al.  Global feature-based attention for motion and color , 2003, Vision Research.

[55]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  Control of object-based attention in human cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  Erik Blaser,et al.  Tracking an object through feature space , 2000, Nature.

[58]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[60]  A. Treisman,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.

[61]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  S. Zeki The visual association cortex , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[63]  H. Lüders,et al.  American Electroencephalographic Society Guidelines for Standard Electrode Position Nomenclature , 1991, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[64]  S. Tipper,et al.  Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[65]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[66]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  Preparatory neural activity predicts performance on a conflict task , 2007, Brain Research.

[68]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[69]  F. Golla The Central Nervous System , 1960, Nature.

[70]  S. Luck,et al.  Spatio‐temporal dynamics of attention to color: Evidence from human electrophysiology , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[71]  A. Treisman,et al.  The Stroop Test: Selective Attention to Colours and Words , 1969, Nature.

[72]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[73]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[74]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Task-dependent field potentials in human hippocampal formation , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[75]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[76]  H J Müller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[77]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Selective attention to color and location: An analysis with event-related brain potentials , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[78]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[79]  Paul Maruff,et al.  Behavioral Goals Constrain the Selection of Visual Information , 1999 .

[80]  S. Pollmann,et al.  A Fronto-Posterior Network Involved in Visual Dimension Changes , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[81]  G. Baylis,et al.  Visual attention and objects: evidence for hierarchical coding of location. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  J. Knott,et al.  Regarding the American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature: a commentary on the proposal to change the 10-20 electrode designators. , 1993, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[83]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Visual activation of frontal cortex: segregation from occipital activity. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[84]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[85]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[86]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[87]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[88]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[89]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  State dependent activity in monkey visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[90]  B. C. Motter,et al.  Neural correlates of feature selective memory and pop-out in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[91]  G. Boynton,et al.  Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[92]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[93]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[94]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[95]  R. Knight,et al.  Mechanisms of human attention: event-related potentials and oscillations , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[96]  H-J Heinze,et al.  Unmasking Motion-Processing Activity in Human Brain Area V5/MT+ Mediated by Pathways That Bypass Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, NeuroImage.

[97]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The ɛ-Adjustment Procedure for Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance , 1976 .

[98]  R. Verleger On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[99]  C Miniussi,et al.  Orienting attention in time. , 2001, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[100]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Spatial Attention Effects in Macaque Area V4 , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[101]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .

[102]  L M Optican,et al.  Primate striate and prestriate cortical neurons during discrimination. I. simultaneous temporal encoding of information about color and pattern. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[103]  Katie L. McMahon,et al.  Classic identity negative priming involves accessing semantic representations in the left anterior temporal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[104]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[105]  Kenneth A. Kooi,et al.  American electroencephalographic society , 1964 .

[106]  Charles L. Folk,et al.  A Dissociation Between Attention and Selection , 2001, Psychological science.

[107]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[108]  D. C. Van Essen,et al.  Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[109]  Pieter R. Roelfsema,et al.  Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey , 1998, Nature.

[110]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[111]  S. P. Wise,et al.  Primate frontal cortex: neuronal activity following attentional versus intentional cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[112]  M Corbetta,et al.  Selective attention modulates extrastriate visual regions in humans during visual feature discrimination and recognition. , 1991, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[113]  M. Valdés-Sosa,et al.  Switching Attention without Shifting the Spotlight: Object-Based Attentional Modulation of Brain Potentials , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[114]  I. Rock,et al.  Perceptual organization and attention , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[115]  Asher Cohen,et al.  Perceptual Dimensional Constraints in Response Selection Processes , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[116]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[117]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of color processing in human visual cortex. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[118]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.