The Ponzo Illusion with Auditory Substitution of Vision in Sighted and Early-Blind Subjects

We tested the effects of using a prosthesis for substitution of vision with audition (PSVA) on sensitivity to the Ponzo illusion. The effects of visual experience on the susceptibility to this illusion were also assessed. In one experiment, both early-blind and blindfolded sighted volunteers used the PSVA to explore several variants of the Ponzo illusion as well as control stimuli. No effects of the illusion were observed. The results indicate that subjects focused their attention on the two central horizontal bars of the stimuli, without processing the contextual cues that convey perspective in the Ponzo figure. In a second experiment, we required subjects to use the PSVA to consider the two converging oblique lines of the stimuli before comparing the length of the two horizontal bars. Here we were able to observe susceptibility to the Ponzo illusion in the sighted group, but to a lesser extent than in a sighted non-PSVA control group. No clear effect of the ilusion was obtained in early-blind subjects. These results suggest that, at least in sighted subjects, perception obtained with the PSVA shares perceptual processes with vision. Visual experience appears mandatory for a Ponzo illusion to occur with the PSVA.

[1]  P Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Transmission of pictorial information through the skin. , 1973, Advances in biological and medical physics.

[2]  H W Leibowitz,et al.  The relation between age and the magnitude of the Ponzo illusion. , 1967, The American journal of psychology.

[3]  G. H. Fisher An Experimental and Theoretical Appraisal of the Perspective and Size-Constancy Theories of Illusions , 1970, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  W.J. Tompkins,et al.  Electrotactile and vibrotactile displays for sensory substitution systems , 1991, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[5]  G. Stack,et al.  Molyneux's Question , 1979 .

[6]  K Suzuki,et al.  Geometrical haptic illusions revisited: Haptic illusions compared with visual illusions , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  P. Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Form perception with a 49-point electrotactile stimulus array on the tongue: a technical note. , 1998, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[8]  S Appelle,et al.  Effect of Modality-Specific Experience on Visual and Haptic Judgment of Orientation , 1985, Perception.

[9]  R. Held Dissociation of visual functions by deprivation and rearrangement , 1968 .

[10]  R. Gregory,et al.  Distortion of Visual Space as Inappropriate Constancy Scaling , 1963, Nature.

[11]  System der optischen und haptischen Raumtäuschungen. , 1934 .

[12]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[13]  R. Day Visual Spatial Illusions: A General Explanation , 1972, Science.

[14]  J. Campos,et al.  Cardiac Responses on the Visual Cliff in Prelocomotor Human Infants , 1970, Science.

[15]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans , 1999, Nature.

[16]  I. Rock,et al.  An introduction to perception , 1975 .

[17]  David Travieso,et al.  Haptic Perception of Geometric Illusions by Persons who are Totally Congenitally Blind , 1999 .

[18]  Reinhard Tausch,et al.  Optische Täuschungen als artifizielle Effekte der Gestaltungsprozesse von Größen- und Formenkonstanz in der natürlichen Raumwahrnehmung , 1954 .

[19]  C. Trullemans,et al.  A real-time experimental prototype for enhancement of vision rehabilitation using auditory substitution , 1998, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[20]  P. Bach-y-Rita Brain mechanisms in sensory substitution , 1972 .

[21]  M. Wanet-Defalque,et al.  Auditory coding of visual patterns for the blind. , 1999, Perception.

[22]  E. Gibson,et al.  The "visual cliff". , 1960, Scientific American.

[23]  Charles E. Rice,et al.  Perceptual Enhancement in the Early Blind? , 1969 .

[24]  Marie-Chantal Wanet-Defalque,et al.  Auditory substitution of vision: pattern recognition by the blind , 2001 .

[25]  J. Rauschecker Compensatory plasticity and sensory substitution in the cerebral cortex , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Morton A. Heller,et al.  Touch, representation, and blindness , 2000 .

[27]  J. Frisby,et al.  Is the Haptic Müller–Lyer a Visual Phenomenon? , 1971, Nature.

[28]  Paul Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Brain plasticity: ‘visual’ acuity of blind persons via the tongue , 2001, Brain Research.

[29]  M. Heller,et al.  Production and Interpretation of Perspective Drawings by Blind and Sighted People , 1996, Perception.

[30]  J. G. Wallace,et al.  Recovery from early blindness : a case study , 1963 .

[31]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental imagery and sensory experience in congenital blindness , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[33]  A Yonas,et al.  Development of sensitivity to pictorial depth. , 1978, Science.

[34]  K. Kaczmarek Sensory Augmentation and Substitution , 1999 .

[35]  A Yonas,et al.  Sensitivity to binocular depth information in infants. , 1976, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[36]  Charles H. Bean The blind have "optical illusions." , 1938 .

[37]  G. Brindley “Seeing” with Skin , 1973, Nature.

[38]  Peter B. L. Meijer,et al.  An experimental system for auditory image representations , 1992, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[39]  L. S. Tsai Mueller-Lyer Illusion by the Blind , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[40]  BENJAMIN WHITE,et al.  Vision Substitution by Tactile Image Projection , 1969, Nature.

[41]  Sandra M. Sanabria-Bohórquez,et al.  Occipital Activation by Pattern Recognition in the Early Blind Using Auditory Substitution for Vision , 2001, NeuroImage.