Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis

While there is ample evidence that children treat words as mutually exclusive, the cognitive basis of this bias is widely debated. We focus on the distinction between pragmatic and lexical constraints accounts. High-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) offer a unique perspective on this debate, as they acquire substantial vocabularies despite impoverished social-pragmatic skills. We tested children and adolescents with ASD in a paradigm examining mutual exclusivity for words and facts. Words were interpreted contrastively more often than facts. Word performance was associated with vocabulary size; fact performance was associated with social-communication skills. Thus mutual exclusivity does not appear to be driven by pragmatics, suggesting that it is either a lexical constraint or a reflection of domain-general learning processes.

[1]  F. Happé Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory , 1993, Cognition.

[2]  D. Behrend,et al.  Learning words from reliable and unreliable speakers. , 2008 .

[3]  K. Loveland,et al.  Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay , 1986, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  J. Reznick,et al.  Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[5]  D. K. Nelson,et al.  Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter , 2000, Cognition.

[6]  A. Woodward,et al.  Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[7]  Mark A. Sabbagh,et al.  How children block learning from ignorant speakers , 2009, Cognition.

[8]  M. Sigman,et al.  Relations Among Joint Attention, Amount of Intervention and Language Gain in Autism , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  S. Waxman,et al.  Taking stock as theories of word learning take shape. , 2008, Developmental science.

[11]  S. Carey,et al.  The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: Evidence from autism , 2005, Cognition.

[12]  John H. Flavell,et al.  Development of the appearance-reality distinction , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Early Word Learning , 1997 .

[14]  L. Bennetto,et al.  Grammaticality judgments in autism: Deviance or delay* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  Terry Regier,et al.  The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's Reliance on Creator's Intent in Extending Names for Artifacts , 2003, Psychological science.

[17]  Jonathan M. Campbell,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test , 2010 .

[18]  정성훈 DIAGNOSTIC AND STATISTIC MANUAL OF MENTAL DISORDERS , 2013 .

[19]  M. Rutter,et al.  Understanding intention in normal development and in autism , 1998 .

[20]  Sally Ozonoff,et al.  Repetitive Behavior Profiles in Asperger Syndrome and High-Functioning Autism , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[21]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[22]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders , 1987 .

[23]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[24]  J. Russell,et al.  Language Profiles in Children with Autism , 1997 .

[25]  E. Markman,et al.  Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning , 1994 .

[26]  B. Pennington,et al.  Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. , 1996, Child development.

[27]  P. Spencer,et al.  Word-learning skills of deaf preschoolers: the development of novel mapping and rapid word-learning strategies. , 2000, Child development.

[28]  E. Clark On the logic of contrast. , 1988, Journal of child language.

[29]  Willard Van Orman Quine,et al.  Word and Object , 1960 .

[30]  C. Mervis,et al.  Early object labels: the case for a developmental lexical principles framework , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[31]  H. Benedict,et al.  Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[32]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[33]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Word learning in a special population: do individuals with Williams syndrome obey lexical constraints? , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[34]  S. Barker,et al.  On the new Riddle of induction , 1960 .

[35]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  G. Marcus,et al.  What developmental disorders can tell us about the nature and origins of language , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Jason Scofield,et al.  Two-year-olds differentially disambiguate novel words and facts* , 2007, Journal of Child Language.

[38]  Patricia A Carpenter,et al.  Verbal and Spatial Working Memory in Autism , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[39]  D. Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Infants' disambiguation of novel object words , 1998 .

[40]  P. Hagoort,et al.  Pragmatic Inferences in High-Functioning Adults with Autism and Asperger Syndrome , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[41]  R. Krauss,et al.  Concurrent feedback, confirmation, and the encoding of referents in verbal communication. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Evidence for a form of mutual exclusivity during label acquisition by grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus)? , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[43]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. , 1996, Child development.

[44]  W. Merriman,et al.  Bilingual and monolingual children's use of two lexical acquisition heuristics , 1993, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[45]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[46]  D. Davidson,et al.  Monolingual and bilingual children's use of the mutual exclusivity constraint , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[47]  J. Bertoncini,et al.  Before and after the vocabulary spurt: two modes of word acquisition? , 2003 .

[48]  C. Mervis,et al.  Two-year-olds readily learn multiple labels for the same basic-level category. , 1994, Child development.

[49]  E. Markman Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. , 1992 .

[50]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Cognition, Perception, and Language , 1997 .

[51]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? , 1997, Child development.

[52]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[53]  M. Sabbagh Communicative Intentions and Language: Evidence from Right-Hemisphere Damage and Autism , 1999, Brain and Language.

[54]  J. Macnamara Names for Things: A Study in Human Learning , 1984 .

[55]  P. Harris,et al.  Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements , 2004, Psychological science.

[56]  Andrew Pickles,et al.  Trajectory of Language Development in Autistic Spectrum Disorders , 2004 .

[57]  Fei Xu The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy , 2002, Cognition.

[58]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The Role of Discourse Novelty in Early Word Learning , 1996 .

[59]  E. Markman,et al.  Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[60]  A. Papafragou,et al.  Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface , 2003, Cognition.

[61]  E. Clark On the pragmatics of contrast , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[62]  Leslie M. Bailey,et al.  Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn New Nouns , 1992 .

[63]  N. Katsos,et al.  Are children with Specific Language Impairment competent with the pragmatics and logic of quantification? , 2011, Cognition.

[64]  Manuela Friedrich,et al.  Early Word Learning , 2010 .

[65]  J. Call,et al.  Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping" , 2004, Science.

[66]  D. Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Infants' Ability to Distinguish Between Intentional and Accidental Actions and Its Relation to Internal State Language. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[67]  W. Stone,et al.  Fast-mapping in young children with autism spectrum disorders , 2006 .

[68]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[69]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. , 2007, Child development.

[70]  J Townsend,et al.  Visual attention abnormalities in autism: Delayed orienting to location , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[71]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[72]  Deirdre Wilson,et al.  Scalar Inferences in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2010, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[73]  T. Au,et al.  The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor? , 1990, Child development.

[74]  G. Diesendruck The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: an empirical examination. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[75]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[76]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  WHAT'S IN A WORD? ON THE CHILD'S ACQUISITION OF SEMANTICS IN HIS FIRST LANGUAGE , 1973 .

[77]  J. Snedeker,et al.  Semantic meaning and pragmatic interpretation in 5-year-olds: evidence from real-time spoken language comprehension. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[78]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The emergence of language. , 1999 .

[79]  J. Halberda,et al.  The development of a word-learning strategy , 2003, Cognition.

[80]  E. Bates,et al.  Language comprehension in ape and child. , 1993, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[81]  H. Tager-Flusberg Defining language phenotypes in autism , 2006, Clinical Neuroscience Research.

[82]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[83]  L. Bennetto,et al.  Beyond Pragmatics: Morphosyntactic Development in Autism , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[84]  Lori Markson,et al.  Two-year-olds' sensitivity to speakers' intent: an alternative account of Samuelson and Smith. , 2004, Developmental science.

[85]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups , 2001, Language and cognitive processes.

[86]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Processes of language acquisition in children with autism: evidence from preferential looking. , 2007, Child development.

[87]  P. Mundy,et al.  A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children , 1990, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[88]  S. Wainwright The importance of shape , 1988 .

[89]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[90]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[91]  D. Tell,et al.  Monolingual and bilingual children's use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[92]  Dante Cicchetti,et al.  Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[93]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's sensitivity to the conventionality of sources. , 2010, Child development.

[94]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Using Speakers ’ Referential Intentions to Model Early Cross-Situational Word Learning , 2022 .

[95]  R. Jeffrey Goodman's Query , 1966 .

[96]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[97]  I. Noveck When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature , 2001, Cognition.

[98]  J. Halberda,et al.  Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[99]  E. Markman,et al.  Rapid Word Learning in 13- and 18-Month-Olds. , 1994 .

[100]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Two-Year-Olds Use Pragmatic Cues to Differentiate Reference to Objects and Actions , 1995 .

[101]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[102]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[103]  Andrew P Bayliss,et al.  Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. , 2005, British journal of psychology.

[104]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[105]  S. Bryson,et al.  Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[106]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[107]  Fei Xu,et al.  Labeling Guides Object Individuation in 12-Month-Old Infants , 2005, Psychological science.

[108]  D. Slobin The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , 1987 .

[109]  F. Moore Cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .

[110]  Jane E. Roberts,et al.  Research on fragile X syndrome and autism: Implications for the study of genes, environments, and developmental language disorders , 2004 .

[111]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children , 1986 .

[112]  T. Regier Emergent constraints on word-learning: a computational perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[113]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Adults Don't Always Know Best , 2006, Psychological science.

[114]  Michael P Maratsos,et al.  Modularity and constraints in language and cognition , 1992 .