Right temporoparietal cortex activation during visuo-proprioceptive conflict.

The conflict between vision and proprioception has been proposed to explain why healthy subjects perform worse than proprioceptively deafferented patients in conditions with optical displacement, e.g. novel mirror drawing. It is not known which brain processes depend upon the successful integration of visual and proprioceptive information and are therefore impaired when these modalities disagree. With fMRI in healthy subjects we compared brain activity across two conditions with similar visual and proprioceptive stimulation and similar task demands that differed by the congruence of movement showed by the two modalities. Subjects felt the passive movement of the right index finger on a rectangular field and watched a cursor moving on a computer screen. Cursor and finger locations either mapped onto each other (congruent condition) or did not (incongruent condition). Monitoring incongruent compared with congruent movement activated the premotor area bilaterally and the right temporoparietal junction. These brain areas have previously been associated with shifts in the attended location in the visual space. These findings suggest an interaction between vision and proprioception in orienting to spatial locations.

[1]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Lack of conscious recognition of one's own actions in a haptically deafferented patient , 2002, Neuroreport.

[2]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  A B Vallbo,et al.  Directional tuning of human forearm muscle afferents during voluntary wrist movements , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  G. Vallar Extrapersonal Visual Unilateral Spatial Neglect and Its Neuroanatomy , 2001, NeuroImage.

[7]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involuntary orienting to sound improves visual perception , 2000, Nature.

[8]  C. Spence,et al.  Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[13]  J Blouin,et al.  Adaptation in visuomanual tracking depends on intact proprioception. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  C. Spence,et al.  Cross-modal links in spatial attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  Jon Driver,et al.  Crossmodal attention , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  Elisabetta Làdavas,et al.  Seeing where your hands are , 1997, Nature.

[17]  Jon Driver,et al.  Attentional competition between modalities: extinction between touch and vision after right hemisphere damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[19]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[20]  Lamarre,et al.  Self-moved target eye tracking in control and deafferented subjects: roles of arm motor command and proprioception in arm-eye coordination. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  N Teasdale,et al.  Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects , 1992, Neurology.

[22]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[23]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[24]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.