Using Peripersonal Warning Signals to Orient a Driver’s Gaze

Objective: We report a series of three experiments designed to assess the relative speed with which people can initiate speeded head-orienting responses following the presentation of spatial warning signals. Background: Recent cognitive neuroscience findings have shown that the human brain tends to treat stimuli occurring in peripersonal space as being somehow more behaviorally relevant and attention demanding than stimuli occurring in extrapersonal space. These brain mechanisms may be exploited in the design of warning signals. Method: Experiment 1 assessed the effectiveness of various different unisensory warning signals in eliciting a head-turning response to look at the potential source of danger requiring participants’ immediate attention; Experiment 2 assessed the latency of a driver’s responses to events occurring in the cued direction; Experiment 3 assessed the relative effectiveness of various warning signals in reorienting a person’s gaze back to a central driving task while he or she was distracted by a secondary task. Results: The results show that participants initiated head-turning movements and made speeded discrimination or braking responses significantly more rapidly following the presentation of a close rear auditory warning signal than following the presentation of either a far frontal auditory warning signal, a vibrotactile warning signal presented to their waist, or a peripheral visual warning signal. Conclusion: These results support the claim that the introduction of peripersonal warning signals results in a significant performance advantage relative to traditionally designed warnings. Application: Warning systems that have been designed around the constraints of the human brain offer great potential in the future design of multisensory interfaces.

[1]  Daniel V. McGehee,et al.  Collision Warning Timing, Driver Distraction, and Driver Response to Imminent Rear-End Collisions in a High-Fidelity Driving Simulator , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[2]  Cristy Ho,et al.  Multisensory In-Car Warning Signals for Collision Avoidance , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[3]  Hong Z. Tan,et al.  Using spatial vibrotactile cues to direct visual attention in driving scenes , 2005 .

[4]  Cristy Ho,et al.  Tactile and Multisensory Spatial Warning Signals for Drivers , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[5]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  The Space Around Us , 1997, Science.

[6]  Alan H S Chan,et al.  Synchronous and asynchronous presentations of auditory and visual signals: Implications for control console design. , 2006, Applied ergonomics.

[7]  Robert Sommer,et al.  Invasions of Personal Space , 1966 .

[8]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[9]  C Spence,et al.  Multisensory interface design for drivers: past, present and future , 2008, Ergonomics.

[10]  Charles Spence,et al.  Audiotactile interactions in near and far space , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Micah M. Murray,et al.  Auditory–somatosensory multisensory interactions in front and rear space , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  C. Spence,et al.  Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration , 2004 .

[13]  F.H. Previc,et al.  Neuropsychological guidelines for aircraft control stations , 2000, IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine.

[14]  Robert Gray,et al.  A Comparison of Tactile, Visual, and Auditory Warnings for Rear-End Collision Prevention in Simulated Driving , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[15]  C. Spence,et al.  The differential effect of vibrotactile and auditory cues on visual spatial attention , 2006, Ergonomics.

[16]  M. J. Horowitz,et al.  BODY-BUFFER ZONE; EXPLORATION OF PERSONAL SPACE. , 1964, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  H. Hediger,et al.  Studies of the psychology and behaviour of captive animals in zoos and circuses. , 1956 .

[18]  Nadine B. Sarter,et al.  Good Vibrations: Tactile Feedback in Support of Attention Allocation and Human-Automation Coordination in Event-Driven Domains , 1999, Hum. Factors.

[19]  Masami Ishihara,et al.  Touch perception reveals the dominance of spatial over digital representation of numbers , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  C. Spence Audiovisual multisensory integration , 2007 .

[21]  Cristy Ho,et al.  Verbal interface design: Do verbal directional cues automatically orient visual spatial attention? , 2006, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[22]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Daniel V. McGehee,et al.  Effect of Warning Timing on Collision Avoidance Behavior in a Stationary Lead Vehicle Scenario , 2002 .

[24]  J. Findlay,et al.  Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  E. Freedman Coordination of the eyes and head during visual orienting , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .

[27]  C. Spence,et al.  Assessing the effectiveness of various auditory cues in capturing a driver's visual attention. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[28]  M. Dosey,et al.  Personal space and self-protection. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  James P Bliss,et al.  Alarm mistrust in automobiles: how collision alarm reliability affects driving. , 2003, Applied ergonomics.

[30]  Jon Driver,et al.  A new approach to the design of multimodal warning signals , 1999 .

[31]  Thomas K. Ferris,et al.  Cross-Modal Links Among Vision, Audition, and Touch in Complex Environments , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[32]  Cristy Ho,et al.  Assessing the effectiveness of "intuitive" vibrotactile warning signals in preventing front-to-rear-end collisions in a driving simulator. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[33]  Luis C. Populin,et al.  Human sound localization: measurements in untrained, head-unrestrained subjects using gaze as a pointer , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Simon Carlile,et al.  Compression of auditory space during rapid head turns , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  Auditory–somatosensory multisensory interactions are spatially modulated by stimulated body surface and acoustic spectra , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  John Duncan,et al.  The demonstration of capacity limitation , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Charles Spence,et al.  Audiotactile multisensory interactions in human information processing , 2006 .