Repeated Administration Effects on Psychomotor Vigilance Test Performance

Study Objectives The Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) is reported to be free of practice effects that can otherwise confound the effects of sleep loss and circadian misalignment on performance. This differentiates the PVT from more complex cognitive tests. To the best of our knowledge, no study has systematically investigated practice effects on the PVT across multiple outcome domains, depending on administration interval, and in ecologically more valid settings. Methods We administered a validated 3-minute PVT (PVT-B) 16 times in 45 participants (23 male, mean ± SD age 32.6 ± 7.3 years, range 25-54 years) with administration intervals of ≥10 days, ≤5 days, or 4 times per day. We investigated linear and logarithmic trends across repeated administrations in 10 PVT-B outcome variables. Results The fastest 10% of response times (RT; plin = .0002), minimum RT (plog = .0010), and the slowest 10% of reciprocal RT (plog = .0124) increased while false starts (plog = 0.0050) decreased with repeated administration, collectively decreasing RT variability (plog = .0010) across administrations. However, the observed absolute changes were small (e.g., -0.03 false starts per administration, linear fit) and are probably irrelevant in practice. Test administration interval did not modify the effects of repeated administration on PVT-B performance (all p > .13 for interaction). Importantly, mean and median RT, response speed, and lapses, which are among the most frequently used PVT outcomes, did not change systematically with repeated administration. Conclusions PVT-B showed stable performance across repeated administrations. Combined with its high sensitivity, this corroborates the status of the PVT as the de facto gold standard measure of the neurobehavioral effects of sleep loss and circadian misalignment.

[1]  Becky S. Weimer,et al.  IEEE Visualization Cumulative Bibiography (1990 - 2004) , 2004, Definitions.

[2]  G. Plazzi,et al.  REM Sleep EEG Instability in REM Sleep Behavior Disorder and Clonazepam Effects , 2017, Sleep.

[3]  Dylan S. Small,et al.  Sleep and Alertness in Medical Interns and Residents: An Observational Study on the Role of Extended Shifts , 2017, Sleep.

[4]  D. Dinges,et al.  A new likelihood ratio metric for the psychomotor vigilance test and its sensitivity to sleep loss , 2015, Journal of sleep research.

[5]  Adrian J. Ecker,et al.  Development and Validation of the Cognition Test Battery for Spaceflight. , 2015, Aerospace medicine and human performance.

[6]  J. Dorrian,et al.  What happens to mood, performance and sleep in a laboratory study with no sleep deprivation? , 2013, Sleep and biological rhythms.

[7]  Adrian J. Ecker,et al.  Mars 520-d mission simulation reveals protracted crew hypokinesis and alterations of sleep duration and timing , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Dylan S. Small,et al.  Effect of a protected sleep period on hours slept during extended overnight in-hospital duty hours among medical interns: a randomized trial. , 2012, JAMA.

[9]  D. Dinges,et al.  Validity and Sensitivity of a Brief Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT-B) to Total and Partial Sleep Deprivation. , 2011, Acta astronautica.

[10]  D. Dinges,et al.  Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. , 2011, Sleep.

[11]  John A. Detre,et al.  Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect , 2010, NeuroImage.

[12]  K. Gluck,et al.  Fatigue in Sustained Attention: Generalizing Mechanisms for Time Awake to Time on Task , 2010 .

[13]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[14]  D. Dinges,et al.  Sleep Deprivation and Vigilant Attention , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[15]  P. Maruff,et al.  Practice Effects Associated with the Repeated Assessment of Cognitive Function Using the CogState Battery at 10-minute, One Week and One Month Test-retest Intervals , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[16]  E. Siemers,et al.  Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[17]  D. Dinges,et al.  Sleep, circadian rhythms, and psychomotor vigilance. , 2005, Clinics in sports medicine.

[18]  T. Mulrooney,et al.  Sleep Deprivation: Clinical Issues, Pharmacology, and Sleep Loss Effects , 2005 .

[19]  D. Dinges,et al.  Psychomotor Vigilance Performance: Neurocognitive Assay Sensitive to Sleep Loss , 2004 .

[20]  R. Chervin Epworth sleepiness scale? , 2003, Sleep medicine.

[21]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[22]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose‐response study , 2003, Journal of sleep research.

[23]  S. Doran,et al.  Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. , 2001, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[24]  D Curran-Everett,et al.  Multiple comparisons: philosophies and illustrations. , 2000, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[25]  Melissa M. Mallis,et al.  MANAGING FATIGUE BY DROWSINESS DETECTION: CAN TECHNOLOGICAL PROMISES BE REALIZED? , 1998 .

[26]  A. Pack,et al.  Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. , 1997, Sleep.

[27]  Robert E. Schlegel,et al.  Microgravity Effects on Cognitive Performance Measures: Practice Schedules to Acquire and Maintain Performance Stability. , 1995 .

[28]  D. Dinges,et al.  Vigilance decrement and sleepiness. , 1994 .

[29]  M. Johns,et al.  A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. , 1991, Sleep.

[30]  D. Dinges,et al.  Performing while sleepy: Effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. , 1991 .

[31]  H. Bozdogan Model selection and Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC): The general theory and its analytical extensions , 1987 .

[32]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[33]  Satterthwaite Fe An approximate distribution of estimates of variance components. , 1946 .

[34]  F. E. Satterthwaite An approximate distribution of estimates of variance components. , 1946, Biometrics.