Perceived reachability: the roles of handedness and hemifield

The impact of reaching experience on the ability to predict one’s own reaching range was investigated. Left- and right-handed participants made verbal estimates about the reachability of a target object for both arms in ipsilateral and contralateral frontal space. There was a significant overestimation bias in both groups and for both hands. The overestimation bias increased with the target object’s eccentricity in contralateral space. The implications of these findings for models of motor control and motor imagery are discussed.

[1]  P. Rochat,et al.  An account of the systematic error in judging what is reachable. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J. D. Fisk,et al.  The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  S. H. Johnson,et al.  Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension , 2000, Cognition.

[4]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  C. Gross,et al.  Spatial maps for the control of movement , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  S. Robinovitch,et al.  Perception of postural limits during reaching. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  Reinoud J. Bootsma,et al.  The Effects of Anxiety on Perceiving the Reachability of Passing Objects , 1992 .

[8]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.

[9]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  J. Stins,et al.  A kinematic analysis of hand selection in a reaching task , 2001, Laterality.

[11]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Speed and sequential effects in reaching. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[13]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[14]  Loukia D. Loukopoulos,et al.  Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. , 1995, Psychological review.

[15]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  M. Bryden,et al.  A behavioral measure of hand preference as opposed to hand skill , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  H. Barlow Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology by Stephen E. Palmer , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  D. Sugden,et al.  Movement Skill Development , 1985 .

[20]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. , 2001, Psychological review.

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[22]  M. Jeannerod The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Postural dynamics and the preferred critical boundary for visually guided reaching. , 1997, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[24]  Y Trotter,et al.  Distance Perception within near Visual Space , 2001, Perception.

[25]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Motor and Visual Imagery as Two Complementary but Neurally Dissociable Mental Processes , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  P. Maruff,et al.  Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominanthands in real and imagined motor task performance , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Estimating reachability: Whole body engagement or postural stability? , 2000 .

[28]  Martin H Fischer Can we correctly perceive the reaching range of others? , 2003, British journal of psychology.

[29]  Francesca Frassinetti,et al.  Direct evidence from parietal extinction of enhancement of visual attention near a visible hand , 2000, Current Biology.

[30]  N. Hogan,et al.  Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements? , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[31]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[32]  M. Annett A classification of hand preference by association analysis. , 1970, British journal of psychology.

[33]  Dennis Velakoulis,et al.  The voluntary control of motor imagery. Imagined movements in individuals with feigned motor impairment and conversion disorder , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Chiang-shan Ray Li,et al.  Impairment of motor imagery in putamen lesions in humans , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  J. L. Myers,et al.  Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.