The influence of briefly presented randomized target motion on the extraretinal component of ocular pursuit.

We assessed the ability to extract velocity information from brief exposure of a moving target and sought evidence that this information could be used to modulate the extraretinal component of ocular pursuit. A step-ramp target motion was initially visible for a brief randomized period of 50, 100, 150, or 200 ms, but then extinguished for a randomized period of 400 or 600 ms before reappearing and continuing along its trajectory. Target speed (5-20 degrees /s), direction (left/right), and intertrial interval (2.7-3.7 s) were also randomized. Smooth eye movements were initiated after about 130 ms and comprised an initial visually dependent component, which reached a peak velocity that increased with target velocity and initial exposure duration, followed by a sustained secondary component that actually increased throughout extinction for 50- and 100-ms initial exposures. End-extinction eye velocity, reflecting extraretinal drive, increased with initial exposure from 50 to 100 ms but remained similar for longer exposures; it was significantly scaled to target velocity for 150- and 200-ms exposures. The results suggest that extraretinal drive is based on a sample of target velocity, mostly acquired during the first 150 ms, that is stored and forms a goal for generating appropriately scaled eye movements during absence of visual input. End-extinction eye velocity was significantly higher when target reappearance was expected than when it was not, confirming the importance of expectation in generating sustained smooth movement. However, end-extinction eye displacement remained similar irrespective of expectation, suggesting that the ability to use sampled velocity information to predict future target displacement operates independently of the control of smooth eye movement.

[1]  G. Barnes,et al.  Predictive velocity estimation in the pursuit reflex response to pseudo‐random and step displacement stimuli in man. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  Simon J Bennett,et al.  Predictive smooth ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Smooth ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of an accelerating visual target: the role of reflexive and voluntary control , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Stephen G. Lisberger,et al.  A model of visually-guided smooth pursuit eye movements based on behavioral observations , 1994, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[5]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  W. Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Information about Motion Direction in Visual Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  L. Mitrani,et al.  Pursuit eye movements of a disappearing moving target , 1978, Vision Research.

[8]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  G. Barnes,et al.  Predicting the duration of ocular pursuit in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Effects of learning on smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  G. Barnes,et al.  Human ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  H. J. Wyatt,et al.  Offset dynamics of human smooth pursuit eye movements: Effects of target presence and subject attention , 1997, Vision Research.

[13]  C. J. S. Collins,et al.  The occluded onset pursuit paradigm: prolonging anticipatory smooth pursuit in the absence of visual feedback , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  J. L. Gordon,et al.  A model of the smooth pursuit eye movement system , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[15]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Timing the anticipatory recovery in smooth ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Fuchs Saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements in the monkey , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  F A Miles,et al.  Transitions between pursuit eye movements and fixation in the monkey: dependence on context. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  S. Heywood,et al.  Eye movements and the afterimage. I. Tracking the afterimage. , 1971, Vision research.

[19]  Graham R Barnes,et al.  Predictive smooth pursuit eye movements during identification of moving acuity targets , 1999, Vision Research.

[20]  Vincent P. Ferrera,et al.  Estimating invisible target speed from neuronal activity in monkey frontal eye field , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Prediction in the oculomotor system: smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Evidence for synergy between saccades and smooth pursuit during transient target disappearance. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Target acceleration can be extracted and represented within the predictive drive to ocular pursuit. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Matthias Nagel,et al.  Parametric modulation of cortical activation during smooth pursuit with and without target blanking. An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[25]  G. K. Noorden,et al.  Pursuit movements of normal and amblyopic eyes. An electro-ophthalmographic study. 1. Physiology of pursuit movements. , 1962, American journal of ophthalmology.

[26]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Evidence for object permanence in the smooth-pursuit eye movements of monkeys. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Postsaccadic enhancement of initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.