The mirror neuron system contributes to social responding

Observing other people in action reliably activates a network of regions across the human premotor, inferior parietal and middle temporal cortex (Caspers, Zilles, Laird, & Eickhoff, 2010). The frontal and parietal components of this network are widely believed to contain mirror neurons (Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). The function of the human mirror neuron system (MNS) has been much debated, with a focus on the comprehension (Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010) and prediction (Kilner, 2011; Wilson & Knoblich, 2005) of other people’s actions. This interpretation assumes that the participant is primarily a passive observer (as is often the case during fMRI) and ignores two key factors. First, in real life people do not just observe but rather respond and engage in social interactions. Second, the MNS is embedded within and intimately linked to the motor system. Here, I suggest that an important purpose of the MNS is not to understand or even to predict, but rather to respond, in real-time and in a socially appropriate fashion, to the actions of others (see Fig. 1). This paper reviews three important studies which point to this interpretation. Behavioural evidence for a human MNS is largely derived from studies of automatic imitation (Heyes, 2011). A study in this tradition asked participants to use their left or right hand to form a particular shape (shake-hands/fist/grasp) in response to images of a left or right hand in the same posture (Liepelt, Prinz, & Brass, 2010). When viewing a right-handed fist or grasp, participants made faster responses with their left hand (a mirror image of the stimulus hand) in line with previous results and with the hypothesis of a predictive-MNS. However, when viewing a right-handed shake-hands image participants responded faster with their own right hand, which does not mirror the stimulus and thus is not a predictive response. Rather, this effect is driven by the strongly learnt social response to seeing a handshake offered and responding with the non-mirror hand. This novel result suggests that compatibility effects in automatic imitation can be driven by social response preparation. The finding that faster responses for complementary actions can be seen in certain contexts (van Schie, van Waterschoot, & Bekkering, 2008) also supports the idea that these paradigms tap more than basic mirroring. Many other studies also show strong MNS responses to learnt social cues (Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2007; Heyes, 2011), but do not distinguish if it is the learning or the social is more important. If a major purpose of the MNS is predicting other people’s responses, it would make sense for this system to be tuned specifically to biological motion or human forms (Press, 2011). One recent study directly tested this (Cross, Hamilton, Kraemer, Kelley, & Grafton, 2009). Participants were trained to make foot movements to arrow cues in a dance video game and thenwere scannedwith fMRI while passively watching the stimulus videos. These videos fell into a 2 2 factorial design contrasting human form (arrows alone or arrows superimposed over a dancing human) and training (learnt or novel videos). The analysis showed that premotor cortex responded selectively to the trained cue sequence but did not respond to the presence of a dancing human. This is hard to explain under the hypothesis that predicting human actions is important in the MNS. However the results are compatible with the claim that theMNS is engaged by familiar stimuli that are associated with learnt responses (see also Press et al., 2012). Thus the results favour a social responding account of the MNS over the human action-prediction account. Finally, a recent TMS study provides exquisite evidence that MNS engagement is driven by social reciprocity (Sartori, Bucchioni, & Castiello, 2012). Participants watched videos of action sequences while motor evoked potentials (MEP) were recorded from hand and finger muscles. The videos showed an actor pouring coffee (whole hand grip) or sugar (precision grip) into a set of cups. At the start of the videos, participants showed a large MEP when observing a whole hand grip and a small MEP when observing a precision grip, consistent with the mirroring hypothesis. The key manipulation came

[1]  R. Saxe,et al.  Look at this: the neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[2]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Understanding action beyond imitation: reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain , 2010, NeuroImage.

[5]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions , 2010, Cognition.

[6]  Antonia F. de C. Hamilton,et al.  Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  G. Knoblich,et al.  The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  fMRI Evidence of ‘Mirror’ Responses to Geometric Shapes , 2012, PloS one.

[9]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.

[10]  Clare Press,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Action Observation and Robotic Agents: Learning and Anthropomorphism , 2022 .

[11]  Umberto Castiello,et al.  When emulation becomes reciprocity. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[12]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  The Control of Mimicry by Eye Contact Is Mediated by Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.