Compressive mapping of number to space reflects dynamic encoding mechanisms, not static logarithmic transform

Significance The ability to map numbers onto space is fundamental to measurement and mathematics. The mental “numberline” is an important predictor of math ability, thought to reflect an internal, native logarithmic representation of number, later becoming linearized by education. Here we demonstrate that the nonlinearity results not from a static logarithmic transformation but from dynamic processes that incorporate past history into numerosity judgments. We show strong and significant correlations between the response to the current trial and the magnitude of the previous stimuli and that subjects respond with a weighted average of current and recent stimuli, explaining completely the logarithmic-like nonlinearity. We suggest that this behavior reflects a general strategy akin to predictive coding to cope adaptively with environmental statistics. The mapping of number onto space is fundamental to measurement and mathematics. However, the mapping of young children, unschooled adults, and adults under attentional load shows strong compressive nonlinearities, thought to reflect intrinsic logarithmic encoding mechanisms, which are later “linearized” by education. Here we advance and test an alternative explanation: that the nonlinearity results from adaptive mechanisms incorporating the statistics of recent stimuli. This theory predicts that the response to the current trial should depend on the magnitude of the previous trial, whereas a static logarithmic nonlinearity predicts trialwise independence. We found a strong and highly significant relationship between numberline mapping of the current trial and the magnitude of the previous trial, in both adults and school children, with the current response influenced by up to 15% of the previous trial value. The dependency is sufficient to account for the shape of the numberline, without requiring logarithmic transform. We show that this dynamic strategy results in a reduction of reproduction error, and hence improvement in accuracy.

[1]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Symbol addition by monkeys provides evidence for normalized quantity coding , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  David C. Burr,et al.  Separate Mechanisms for Perception of Numerosity and Density , 2014, Psychological science.

[3]  D. Whitney,et al.  Serial dependence in visual perception , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  How do we convert a number into a finger trajectory? , 2013, Cognition.

[5]  David C Burr,et al.  Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Knowledge on the line: Manipulating beliefs about the magnitudes of symbolic numbers affects the linearity of line estimation tasks , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  Sergei Gepshtein,et al.  Sensory adaptation as optimal resource allocation , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality , 2013, Current Biology.

[9]  David C. Burr,et al.  The effects of cross-sensory attentional demand on subitizing and on mapping number onto space , 2012, Vision Research.

[10]  P. Mamassian,et al.  Predictive Properties of Visual Adaptation , 2012, Current Biology.

[11]  David C. Burr,et al.  Linear mapping of numbers onto space requires attention , 2012, Cognition.

[12]  D. Burr,et al.  Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Petzschner,et al.  Iterative Bayesian Estimation as an Explanation for Range and Regression Effects: A Study on Human Path Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: differentiating between scale and bias. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  J. Henderson,et al.  Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[16]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  David C Burr,et al.  Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[18]  James T Todd,et al.  Are discrimination thresholds a valid measure of variance for judgments of slant from texture? , 2010, Journal of vision.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding under the free-energy principle , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  Modulating Attentional Load Affects Numerosity Estimation: Evidence against a Pre-Attentive Subitizing Mechanism , 2008, PloS one.

[21]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures , 2008, Science.

[22]  J. Todd,et al.  The effects of viewing angle, camera angle, and sign of surface curvature on the perception of three-dimensional shape from texture. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[23]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A Labeled-Line Code for Small and Large Numerosities in the Monkey Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Ernst Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[25]  M. Landy,et al.  The effect of viewpoint on perceived visual roughness. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[26]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[27]  Neil W. Roach,et al.  Resolving multisensory conflict: a strategy for balancing the costs and benefits of audio-visual integration , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[29]  A. Nieder Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[31]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[32]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Understanding AIC and BIC in Model Selection , 2004 .

[33]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  S. Dehaene Single-Neuron Arithmetic , 2002, Science.

[35]  Neuroscience. Single-neuron arithmetic. , 2002, Science.

[36]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[37]  G. Fechner Elemente der Psychophysik , 1998 .

[38]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[39]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  J. Atick,et al.  STATISTICS OF NATURAL TIME-VARYING IMAGES , 1995 .

[41]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[42]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[43]  H. Barlow,et al.  The mechanical mind. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[44]  S. S. Stevens On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.

[45]  J. B.,et al.  The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.

[46]  Ernst Heinrich Weber,et al.  De pulsu, resorptione, auditu et tactu. Annotationes anatomicae et physiologicae , 1834 .