Limb apraxia and the “affordance competition hypothesis”

Limb apraxia, a disorder of higher order motor control, has long been a challenge for clinical assessment and understanding (Leiguarda and Marsden, 2000). The deficits originally described in limb apraxia (Liepmann, 1920) have been classified by the nature of the errors made by the patients leading to, namely, ideational and ideomotor apraxia. The dual stream hypothesis (Goodale and Milner, 1992) has been used to explain these categories: ideational apraxia is thought to relate to a deficit in the concept of a movement (coded in the ventral stream). Patients have difficulty using objects, sequencing actions to interact with them or pantomiming their use. Ideomotor apraxia, on the other hand, is thought to arise from problems in the accurate implementation of movements within the dorsal stream. One of the limitations on understanding apraxia is the failure by the clinical literature to draw on knowledge of the factors determining actions in the environment. Here we emphasize the role of affordance. There is much recent work indicating that our responses to stimuli are strongly influenced by the actions that the objects “afford”, based on their physical properties and the intentions of the actor (e.g., Tucker and Ellis, 1998). The concept of affordance, originally suggested by Gibson (1979) has been incorporated in a recent model of interactive behavior that draws from findings in non-human primates, namely the “affordance competition hypothesis” (Cisek, 2007). This postulates that interactive behavior arises by a process of competition between possible actions elicited by the environment. In this paper we argue that “affordance competition” may play a role in apraxia. We review evidence that at least some aspects of apraxia may reflect an abnormal sensitivity to competition when multiple affordances are present (Riddoch et al., 1998) and/or a poor ability to exert cognitive control over this competition when it occurs. This framework suggests a new way of conceptualizing deficits in apraxia which invites further investigations in the field.

[1]  I. Biederman Perceiving Real-World Scenes , 1972, Science.

[2]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[3]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[4]  J J Hopfield,et al.  Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  K. Heilman,et al.  Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.

[6]  A. Basso,et al.  Historical Perspectives On Neuroanatomical Correlates of Limb Apraxia , 1985 .

[7]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Routes to action: Evidence from apraxia , 1989 .

[8]  K. Heilman,et al.  A Cognitive Neuropsychological Model of Limb Praxis , 1991 .

[9]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  C. Marsden,et al.  The basal ganglia and apraxia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  G. Halliday,et al.  Quantification of cortical atrophy in a case of progressive fluent aphasia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  M A Arbib,et al.  From visual affordances in monkey parietal cortex to hippocampo-parietal interactions underlying rat navigation. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[13]  C. Marsden,et al.  Apraxia in Parkinson's disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy and neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  R. Andersen,et al.  Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.

[15]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  The meaning of meaningless gestures: A study of visuo-imitative apraxia , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[17]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Modeling parietal-premotor interactions in primate control of grasping , 1998, Neural Networks.

[18]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[19]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  R T Knight,et al.  Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia. A kinematic analysis of aiming movements. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[22]  R T Knight,et al.  Neural representations of skilled movement. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[24]  B. Hommel The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .

[25]  C D Marsden,et al.  Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[27]  L. Buxbaum Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action , 2001, Neurocase.

[28]  Roberto Cubelli,et al.  Double Dissociation between Meaningful and Meaningless Gesture Reproduction in Apraxia , 2001, Cortex.

[29]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM) , 2002 .

[30]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for a convergent route model for action , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[31]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[33]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Goal-directed imitation in patients with Ideomotor Apraxia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  安藤 広志,et al.  20世紀の名著名論:David Marr:Vision:a Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2005 .

[35]  A. Wing,et al.  Action modulates object-based selection , 2005, Vision Research.

[36]  Garry Young,et al.  Are different affordances subserved by different neural pathways? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[37]  A. Sirigu,et al.  How we interact with objects: learning from brain lesions , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.

[39]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  How to Define an Object: Evidence from the Effects of Action on Perception and Attention , 2007 .

[41]  Byron M. Yu,et al.  Techniques for extracting single-trial activity patterns from large-scale neural recordings , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  K. Heilman,et al.  The disconnection apraxias , 2008, Cortex.

[43]  S. Aglioti,et al.  Neural Underpinnings of Gesture Discrimination in Patients with Limb Apraxia , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  M. Rushworth,et al.  General Mechanisms for Making Decisions? This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited the Representation of Value and Reward Expectations in Frontal Cortex Reward Prediction Errors and Learning Rates Other Types of Prediction Error , 2022 .

[45]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  Patrizia Fattori,et al.  The Dorsomedial Pathway Is Not Just for Reaching: Grasping Neurons in the Medial Parieto-Occipital Cortex of the Macaque Monkey , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[48]  Laurel J Buxbaum,et al.  Critical brain regions for action recognition: lesion symptom mapping in left hemisphere stroke. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  Ashwin G Ramayya,et al.  A DTI investigation of neural substrates supporting tool use. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  H. Bergman,et al.  Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  Marco Davare,et al.  Decoding Action Intentions in Parietofrontal Circuits , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Systematic assessment of apraxia and functional predictions from the Birmingham Cognitive Screen , 2012, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[53]  R. Ivry,et al.  Dissociating the Role of Prefrontal and Premotor Cortices in Controlling Inhibitory Mechanisms during Motor Preparation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Matthew T. Kaufman,et al.  Neural population dynamics during reaching , 2012, Nature.

[55]  J. Lygeros,et al.  Decision Making I , 2014 .

[56]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Apraxia : The Neuropsychology of Action , 2014 .