Developmental and Gender Differences in Preadolescents' Judgments of the Veracity of Gossip

Hypothetical vignettes were used to examine 384 preadolescents' understanding of gossip in varying circumstances. Children correctly labeled talk about nonpresent others as gossip and considered it inappropriate. Skepticism was higher for gossip than for firsthand information and was greatest in the presence of cues suggesting that speakers were unreliable or harbored ulterior motives. Surprisingly, skepticism of gossip decreased with age. Attributions for speakers' behavior were sensitive to subtle contextual cues, and in the absence of clear motives, children, particularly girls, were likely to assume that gossipers spread false information out of jealousy. Overall, preadolescents appear to have a relatively mature understanding of the limitations of gossip, treat gossip with skepticism, and judge harshly gossipers who spread false gossip.

[1]  Mary S. Norman,et al.  The Role of Evaluator‐Victim Relationships in Children's Evaluations of Peer Provocation , 2001 .

[2]  S. Graham,et al.  Self-blame and peer victimization in middle school: an attributional analysis. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[3]  J. Lewko,et al.  Learning the Rules: The Anatomy of Children's Relationships. , 1997 .

[4]  J. Parker,et al.  Forming, losing, renewing, and replacing friendships: Applying temporal parameters to the assessment of children's friendship experiences. , 1996 .

[5]  S. Harter,et al.  The interpersonal context of emotion: anger with close friends and classmates. , 1996, Child development.

[6]  Ken J. Rotenberg,et al.  Development of a Scale to Measure Individual Differences in Children's Trust-Value Basis of Friendship , 1995 .

[7]  H. Holliday,et al.  Gossip in same-gender and cross-gender friends' conversations , 1995 .

[8]  A. La Greca,et al.  What do children worry about? Worries and their relation to anxiety. , 1995, Child development.

[9]  O. Nevo,et al.  The Development of the Tendency to Gossip Questionnaire: Construct and Concurrent Validation for a Sample of Israeli College Students , 1993 .

[10]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Suspicion and Dispositional Inference , 1993 .

[11]  B. Bower A Child's Theory of Mind , 1993 .

[12]  M. Goodwin He-Said-She-Said: Talk As Social Organization Among Black Children , 1993 .

[13]  Donna Eder,et al.  THE STRUCTURE OF GOSSIP: OPPORTUNITIES AND CONSTRAINTS ON COLLECTIVE EXPRESSION AMONG ADOLESCENTS* , 1991 .

[14]  D. Buss,et al.  Derogation of Competitors , 1990 .

[15]  Dean E. Hewes,et al.  Cognition and social information-gathering strategies: reinterpretation assessment in second-guessing , 1989 .

[16]  K. Rotenberg,et al.  Children's use of a verbal-nonverbal consistency principle to infer truth and lying. , 1989, Child development.

[17]  Jürgen Streeck,et al.  Children's Worlds and Children's Language , 1986 .

[18]  Dean E. Hewes,et al.  Knowing when to 'second-guess': The mindful analysis of messages , 1986 .

[19]  G. Fine The Social Organization of Adolescent Gossip: The Rhetoric of Moral Evaluation , 1986 .

[20]  Charles Pavitt,et al.  “SECOND‐GUESSING”: Message Interpretation in Social Networks , 1985 .

[21]  Jack Levin,et al.  An exploratory analysis of sex differences in gossip , 1985 .

[22]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Age Changes in the Detection of Deception. , 1982 .

[23]  R. Selman The growth of interpersonal understanding : developmental and clinical analyses , 1980 .

[24]  Gary Alan Fine,et al.  Gossip, Gossipers, Gossiping , 1978 .

[25]  Jerry Suls,et al.  Gossip as Social Comparison , 1977 .

[26]  Alexander R. Rysman How the "gossip" became a woman. , 1977 .

[27]  Gary Alan Fine,et al.  Social Components of Children's Gossip , 1977 .

[28]  John B. Haviland,et al.  Gossip as Competition in Zinacantan , 1977 .

[29]  R. Abrahams,et al.  A Performance-Centred Approach to Gossip , 1970 .

[30]  M. Gluckman,et al.  Psychological, Sociological and Anthropological Explanations of Witchcraft and Gossip: A Clarification , 1968 .

[31]  R. Paine What is Gossip About? An Alternative Hypothesis , 1967 .

[32]  L. Murphy,et al.  Social and Emotional Development , 1941 .

[33]  R. Cohen,et al.  Children's evaluations of provocation between peers† , 1997 .

[34]  W. Bukowski,et al.  Friendship and morality: (How) are they related? , 1996 .

[35]  E. Higgins,et al.  The interpersonal context , 1996 .

[36]  W. Hartup,et al.  The Company They Keep: Friendship in Childhood and Adolescence. , 1996 .

[37]  J. Flavell,et al.  Young children's knowledge about thinking. , 1995, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[38]  K. Dodge,et al.  A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. , 1994 .

[39]  K. Lagerspetz,et al.  Do Girls Manipulate and Boys Fight? Developmental Trends in Regard to Direct and Indirect Aggression , 1992 .

[40]  K. Björkqvist,et al.  Of Mice and Women: Aspects of Female Aggression , 1994, Politics and the Life Sciences.

[41]  K. Björkqvist,et al.  5 – The Development of Direct and Indirect Aggressive Strategies in Males and Females , 1992 .

[42]  Penelope Eckert,et al.  Cooperative competition in adolescent “girl talk” , 1990 .

[43]  John M. Gottman,et al.  Social and emotional development in a relational context: Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. , 1989 .

[44]  G. Ladd,et al.  Peer relationships in child development. , 1989 .

[45]  John M. Gottman,et al.  Speculations about social and affective development: Friendship and acquaintanceship through adolescence. , 1986 .

[46]  J. Gottman,et al.  Conversations of friends: Speculations on affective development. , 1986 .

[47]  John M. Gottman,et al.  How Children Become Friends. , 1983 .

[48]  T. Poveda Reputation and the adolescent girl: an analysis. , 1975, Adolescence.