165 rectional (i.e., higher intelligence – higher schizophrenia risk). Further analyses indicate a strong protective effect of intelligence on the risk for schizophrenia, and a smaller negative effect of schizophrenia (risk genes) on intelligence. Aspects of cognition are also impaired in relatives of people with schizophrenia, who take an intermediate position between their affected family member and healthy controls. However, the vulnerability to schizophrenia does not appear to be based in an unlucky familial combination of cognitive and environmental risks. An intriguing registry-based study indicates that schizophrenia risk is predicted by the individual’s deviation from familial cognitive aptitude (i.e., what is expected from educational attainment and IQ in parents and siblings) and not by cognitive dysfunction per se. When cases are matched to controls by educational achievement or IQ, their relatives are found to have better cognitive aptitudes than the corresponding relatives of the controls. These findings point to the existence of a qualitatively different developmental impairment that is associated with schizophrenia risk. A central finding from genome-wide association studies is the link between risk of schizophrenia and the immune system, in particular, the complement system. Studies have identified a new role for complement 4 (C4) in synaptic pruning. Synaptic pruning peaks during adolescence, and is essential for refinement of the CNS and maturation of cognitive abilities. Structurally different variants of C4 genes are associated with differences in C4 expression and with the risk of schizophrenia, supporting the notion that elevated complement activity leading to increased synaptic pruning is a risk factor for schizophrenia. A recent study using patient-derived induced pluripotent stem cells found abnormalities in microglia-like cells and synaptic structures, in addition to increased synaptic pruning in the neuronal cultures. Risk-associated variants of the C4 genes were linked to increased complement uptake in synapses. In line with this, there are indications of poorer memory function linked to increased predicted C4 expression, across patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Prospective studies of early cognitive development in children who later developed schizophrenia showed stable deficits in IQ, language, processing speed and executive functioning from infancy. Verbal deficits appear early and are relatively stable, while impairments in processing speed and executive functions increase during adolescence. The widening gap towards healthy adolescence appears mainly to be based in a developmental lag rather than a loss of acquired functions. Studies on groups considered as clinical high-risk (CHR) for psychosis also find significant cognitive dysfunctions. This is particularly the case for those in the CHR group who later experience transition to psychosis. There are, however, no direct indications of a cognitive decline from the prodrome/highrisk state to the onset of the first episode. The main argument for the initial conceptualization of schizophrenia as a neurodegenerative disorder was the presence of cognitive dysfunction and a deteriorating clinical course. However, first episode studies do not find any associations between the duration of untreated psychosis and cognitive dysfunction. Prospective studies of cognitive trajectories from the first episode onwards also show significant cognitive stability, both in shortand long-term. There are some indications of poorer cognitive development in patients with high illness activity during the first year of treatment, but of limited magnitude and balanced by findings of mod est cognitive improvements in other sub groups. Taken together, our knowledge about cognition in the early phases of schizophrenia strongly supports the notion of a primarily neurodevelopmental basis for cognitive dysfunction. Cognitive problems may serve as additional stressors increasing psychosis risk, while other symptoms of the disorder may add to cognitive problems. However, current data indicate that cognitive dysfunction is neither a cause nor a consequence of the psychotic process but rather a biomarker of underlying neurodevelopmental problems. This notion has important clinical implications: while specific treatments may improve one area of dysfunction (cognition or psychotic symptoms) in adults with schizophrenia, this may not translate to other areas. Preventing additional developmental lags in adolescents at high risk might be one of the most effective ways to prevent significant cognitive dysfunction.
[1]
Michael F. Green,et al.
Nonsocial and social cognition in schizophrenia: current evidence and future directions
,
2019,
World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.
[2]
Philip D. Harvey,et al.
Health status and mobility limitations are associated with residential and employment status in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
,
2017,
Journal of psychiatric research.
[3]
Joseph Firth,et al.
Sedentary behavior and physical activity levels in people with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder: a global systematic review and meta‐analysis
,
2017,
World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.
[4]
R. Levy,et al.
Household availability of ultra-processed foods and obesity in nineteen European countries
,
2017,
Public Health Nutrition.
[5]
Philip D. Harvey,et al.
Twenty‐year progression of body mass index in a county‐wide cohort of people with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder identified at their first episode of psychosis
,
2017,
Bipolar disorders.
[6]
E. Bora,et al.
The relationship between cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and metabolic syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis
,
2016,
Psychological Medicine.
[7]
Philip D. Harvey,et al.
Cognitive deficits, obesity and disability in schizophrenia
,
2012
.
[8]
Philip D. Harvey,et al.
Predicting the severity of everyday functional disability in people with schizophrenia: cognitive deficits, functional capacity, symptoms, and health status
,
2012,
World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.
[9]
Emmanuel Stip,et al.
Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia
,
2004,
Schizophrenia Research.