Agenda 2000: Communication: Language as an implementational device for cognition.

The aim of this paper is to identify the functions of social cognition in a communication context. It is argued that social cognition is for the regulation of adaptive action and makes use of external devices (e.g. language) to implement action. First, the complexity of communication as a social achievement is discussed to prepare the ground for examining the link between language, cognition and communication. The implementational devices (language) of social cognition are addressed next. On the basis of these considerations a message-modulation model is proposed to conceptualize the interplay between language, cognition, motivation and communication. The application of this model to research on the transmission and maintenance of stereotypes illustrates the types of open research issues and directions that may be possible routes for future work. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  D. Wigboldus,et al.  Children's communication of the Linguistic Intergroup Bias and its impact upon cognitive inferences , 1999 .

[2]  S. Fiske,et al.  Confusing one person with another: what errors reveal about the elementary forms of social relations. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  How to learn a language like a chimpanzee , 1990 .

[4]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Accuracy and bias in estimates of others' knowledge , 1991 .

[5]  Roger T. Johnson,et al.  9 – Goal Interdependence and Interpersonal Attraction in Heterogeneous Classrooms: A Metanalysis* , 1984 .

[6]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Social encoding and subsequent attitudes, impressions, and memory: "Context-driven" and motivational aspects of processing. , 1984 .

[7]  Ivy Yee-Man Lau,et al.  Some Cognitive Consequences of Communication , 1998 .

[8]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Understanding friends and strangers: The effects of audience design on message comprehension , 1989 .

[9]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Social cognition: learning about what matters in the social world , 2000 .

[10]  H. Giles,et al.  Language: Contexts and Consequences , 1991 .

[11]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[12]  Jennifer Crocker,et al.  Social Stigma and Self-Esteem: Situational Construction of Self-Worth , 1999 .

[13]  L. Caporael The Evolution of Truly Social Cognition: The Core Configurations Model , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[14]  G. Semin Language and Social Cognition , 2007 .

[15]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Primacy and recency in communication and self-persuasion : how successive audiences and multiple encodings influence subsequent evaluative judgments , 1991 .

[16]  E. Higgins,et al.  The “Communication Game”: Goal-Directed Encoding and Cognitive Consequences , 1982 .

[17]  H. Tajfel,et al.  An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. , 1979 .

[18]  L. Resnick,et al.  Social foundations of cognition. , 1993, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  R. Wyer,et al.  Cognitive representation of conversations about persons. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  G. Semin Interfacing Language and Social Cognition , 1995 .

[21]  H. Kelley,et al.  The social psychology of groups , 1960 .

[22]  E. Hutchins Cognition in the wild , 1995 .

[23]  H. Levin,et al.  An accommodating witness , 1988 .

[24]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range , 1991 .

[25]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Common ground at the understanding of demonstrative reference , 1983 .

[26]  R. Wyer Associated systems theory : a systematic approach to cognitive representations of persons , 1994 .

[27]  D. Wigboldus,et al.  How do we communicate stereotypes? Linguistic bases and inferential consequences. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  A. Maass,et al.  Implicit Versus Explicit Strategies of Out-Group Discrimination , 1996 .

[29]  R. Rommetveit On Message Structure: A Framework for the Study of Language and Communication , 1976, Dance Research Journal.

[30]  G. Semin,et al.  The question–answer paradigm: You might regret not noticing how a question is worded. , 1997 .

[31]  M. Deutsch A Theory of Co-operation and Competition , 1949 .

[32]  S. Fiske,et al.  Thinking is for doing: portraits of social cognition from daguerreotype to laserphoto. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  D. M. Taylor,et al.  Group Processes Affecting Anticipated Language Choice in Intergroup Relations , 1980 .

[34]  Anne Maass,et al.  Linguistic Intergroup Bias: Stereotype Perpetuation Through Language , 1999 .

[35]  H. Giles Towards a theory of language in ethnic group relations , 1977 .

[36]  A. Maass,et al.  Linguistic intergroup bias: Evidence for in-group-protective motivation. , 1996 .

[37]  G. Semin,et al.  Language use in intergroup contexts: the linguistic intergroup bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Bert H. Hodges,et al.  Values as constraints on affordances: Perceiving and acting properly , 1992 .

[39]  N. Coupland Accommodation at work: some phonological data and their implications , 1984 .

[40]  J. Austin How to do things with words , 1962 .

[41]  Donald M. Taylor,et al.  Towards a theory of interpersonal accommodation through language: some Canadian data , 1973, Language in Society.

[42]  W. Doise,et al.  Evaluation of Accent Convergence and Divergence in Cooperative and Competitive Intergroup Situations , 1976 .

[43]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  "Saying is believing": Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. , 1978 .

[44]  A. Maass,et al.  Linguistic intergroup bias: differential expectancies or in-group protection? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  A. Clark,et al.  The Extended Mind , 1998, Analysis.

[46]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. , 1996 .

[47]  G. Semin Language as a Cognitive and Behavioral Structuring Resource: Question—Answer Exchanges , 2000 .

[48]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. , 1988 .

[49]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Asking questions and influencing answers. , 1992 .

[50]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Judgment in a Social Context: Biases, Shortcomings, and the Logic of Conversation , 1994 .

[51]  Stephanie D. Teasley,et al.  Perspectives on socially shared cognition , 1991 .

[52]  Lucian Gideon Conway,et al.  Influence of Impression-Management Goals on the Emerging Contents of Group Stereotypes: Support for a Social-Evolutionary Process , 1999 .

[53]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Social psychological models of interpersonal communication , 1996 .

[54]  Janet B. Ruscher,et al.  Multiple communicators with unique target information transmit less stereotypical impressions , 1998 .

[55]  R. Zajonc The process of cognitive tuning in communication. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[56]  N. Schwarz Agenda 2000 — Social judgment and attitudes: warmer, more social, and less conscious , 2000 .

[57]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Other-Relatedness in Language Processing: Discussion and Comments , 1988 .

[58]  H. Grice Utterer's meaning and intentions , 1969 .

[59]  L. Thompson,et al.  Socially Shared Cognition, Affect, and Behavior: A Review and Integration , 1999, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[60]  H. Grice Further Notes on Logic and Conversation , 1978 .

[61]  G. Semin,et al.  Differential contributions of linguistic factors to memory based ratings: Systematizing the systematic distortion hypothesis , 1985 .

[62]  E. Schegloff,et al.  A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation , 1974 .

[63]  G. Semin Cognition, language, and communication , 1998 .

[64]  H. H. Clark Arenas of language use , 1993 .