The malleability of eyewitness confidence: co-witness and perseverance effects

A theft was staged 70 times for pairs of eyewitnesses (N = 140) who then made a photo-lineup identification. Witnesses then received 1 of 9 types of information regarding the alleged identification decision of their co-witness. Witnesses told that their co-witness identified the same person whom they had identified showed an increase in the confidence they expressed to a confederate police officer. Confidence deflation occurred among witnesses who thought their co-witness either identified another person or had stated that the thief was not in the lineup. Initial co-witness information was not mitigated by subsequent changes to that information. A second study showed videotapes of these witnesses' testimonies to observers (n = 378) whose credibility ratings of the testimony paralleled the witnesses' self-rated confidence. Eyewitness identification confidence is highly malleable after the identification has been made despite the fact that physical resemblance between the culprit and person identified has not changed. If an eyewitness says "I am absolutely confident that he is the guy I saw rob the liquor store," it is rather difficult for people to accept the idea that the witness could be wrong. Numerous studies have demonstrated a close relation between the confidence expressed by an eyewitness and people's propensities to accept that eyewitness's testimony as accurate (e.g., Brigham & Bothwell, 1983; Fox & Walters, 1986; Lindsay, Wells, & Rumpel, 1981; Wells & Leippe, 1981; Wells, Ferguson, & Lindsay, 1981; Wells, Lindsay, & Ferguson, 1979; Yarmey & Jones, 1983). It is probably quite rational to place more trust in the validity of someone's judgment when that person makes a statement with high confidence rather than with little confidence. In the case of eyewitness identification testimony, however, the observed empirical relation between accuracy and confidence is surprisingly weak. A meta-analysis by Bothwell, Deffenbacher, and Brigham (1987) indicates that eyewitness confidence accounts for about 6% of the variance in eyewitness identification accuracy. Hence, it is not uncommon in research to find eyewitnesses making false identifications with high self-rated confidence that they are correct (or, conversely, eyewitnesses being quite unsure and yet accurate in their identifications).

[1]  M. R. Leippe,et al.  How do triers of fact infer the accuracy of eyewitness identifications? Using memory for peripheral detail can be misleading. , 1981 .

[2]  A. D. Pearman,et al.  Judgment and Decision Making: An Interdisciplinary Reader , 1999 .

[3]  M. Sherif An experimental approach to the study of attitudes. , 1937 .

[4]  Lionel Haward,et al.  Evaluating witness evidence: S. Llyod-Bostock and R. Clifford (Eds): Wiley, Chichester, England (1983). x + 305 pages. £19.95. , 1984 .

[5]  M. R. Leippe,et al.  Reactions to child (Versus adult) eyewitnesses , 1989 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[7]  H. A. Walters,et al.  The impact of general versus specific expert testimonyand eyewitness confidence upon mock juror judgment , 1986 .

[8]  Gary L. Wells,et al.  The Tractability of Eyewitness Confidence and Its Implications for Triers of Fact , 1981 .

[9]  S. Asch Opinions and Social Pressure , 1955, Nature.

[10]  G. Wells,et al.  Does Knowledge that a Crime Was Staged Affect Eyewitness Performance?1 , 1982 .

[11]  C. Kiesler The psychology of commitment : experiments linking behavior to belief , 1971 .

[12]  Robert Rosenthal,et al.  Effect sizes: Pearson's correlation, its display via the BESD, and alternative indices. , 1991 .

[13]  G. Wells,et al.  Maximizing the Utility of Eyewitness Identification Evidence , 1994 .

[14]  R. Malpass,et al.  Realism and eyewitness identification research , 1980 .

[15]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Can People Detect Eyewitness-Identification Accuracy Within and Across Situations? , 1981 .

[16]  G. Wells,et al.  Police Lineups as Experiments , 1990 .

[17]  Kenneth A. Deffenbacher,et al.  Correlation of eyewitness accuracy and confidence: Optimality hypothesis revisited. , 1987 .

[18]  G. Wells,et al.  What do we know about eyewitness identification? , 1993, The American psychologist.

[19]  G. Wells Eyewitness identification: A system handbook , 1988 .

[20]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Accuracy, confidence, and juror perceptions in eyewitness identification. , 1979, The Journal of applied psychology.

[21]  Elizabeth F. Loftus,et al.  Eyewitness testimony : psychological perspectives , 1984 .

[22]  Gary L. Wells,et al.  The selection of distractors for eyewitness lineups , 1993 .

[23]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  Endel Tulving,et al.  Similarity relations in recognition , 1981 .

[25]  Gary L. Wells,et al.  Guidelines for empirically assessing the fairness of a lineup , 1979 .

[26]  J. Brigham,et al.  The ability of prospective jurors to estimate the accuracy of eyewitness identifications , 1983 .

[27]  V. L. Allen,et al.  Impact of group consensus and social support on stimulus meaning: Mediation of conformity by cognitive restructuring. , 1980 .

[28]  M. R. Leippe,et al.  Effects of integrative memorial and cognitive processes on the correspondence of eyewitness accuracy and confidence , 1980 .

[29]  L. Ross,et al.  Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  G. Wells The Psychology of Lineup Identifications1 , 1984 .