Modeling High-Stakes Consumer Decisions in Repeated Contexts : The Problem of Mammography Adherence Following False Alarm Test Results

We thank Emily Conant, M.D. for facilitating access to the mammography clinic of the Hospital at the University of Pennsylvania and for verifying the accuracy of our experimental materials. We thank for invaluable help in data collection. Abstract Consumers often have to decide whether to acquire information in stressful, high-stakes domains. We study women in mammography waiting rooms to test how the information-gathering experience influences intentions regarding their next scheduled mammogram. In particular, we investigate how a patient's " false alarm " result on one occasion (i.e., a mammogram's indication that cancer is present when a 'more accurate' follow-up test reveals it is not) affects willingness to get a regularly scheduled mammogram the next time it is due. We adapt the normative value of information framework by hypothesizing that stress may moderate reactions to a false positive test result. Study 1 demonstrates that a false alarm can delay planned testing adherence but only when that false alarm test result has no implications about the likelihood of future cancer. For patients receiving a false alarm result, information providing either problem-focused coping support by stressing the patient's control or emotion-focused coping support by stressing the frequency of false alarm results mitigates negative future adherence effects. These reactions to false alarm results are moderated by stress, a finding that is developed in Study 2 where we show that stress alters patients' assessments of the utilities associated with testing outcomes. Study 3 shows that, in the presence of a false alarm result, women with actual false alarm history who experience stress are more likely to delay their next mammogram whereas women without actual false alarm history who experience stress are less likely to delay their next mammogram. Stress 2 Consumer decisions vary considerably in the magnitude of their potential impact. For instance, the impact of choosing the wrong brand of a consumer package good is likely to be small, while the impact of failing to obtain information on cancer warning signs could literally be of life or death proportions. Clearly, consequential consumer decisions such as those regarding health care, housing, insurance or security involve a substantial proportion of the economy and are likely to occupy a significant proportion of consumers' overall decision efforts. However, despite calls to the contrary), the vast majority of marketing research in modeling consumer choice focuses on decisions with very limited consequences. Consequential decision-making can generate negative emotion or stress, …

[1]  D P Winchester,et al.  Limitations of mammography in the identification of noninfiltrating carcinoma of the breast. , 1988, Surgery, gynecology & obstetrics.

[2]  R. Fazio Multiple Processes by which Attitudes Guide Behavior: The Mode Model as an Integrative Framework , 1990 .

[3]  J. Holland,et al.  Psychological distress and surveillance behaviors of women with a family history of breast cancer. , 1992, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[4]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[5]  E. Pisano,et al.  Screening mammography behavior after a false positive mammogram. , 1998, Cancer detection and prevention.

[6]  The Context Matters , 2000 .

[7]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[8]  E. Weber,et al.  Determinants of diagnostic hypothesis generation: effects of information, base rates, and experience. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Mark R. Lepper,et al.  Social explanation and social expectation: Effects of real and hypothetical explanations on subjective likelihood. , 1977 .

[10]  R. Lazarus Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[11]  J. Baron,et al.  Heuristics and biases in diagnostic reasoning: I. Priors, error costs, and test accuracy , 1988 .

[12]  M R Lepper,et al.  Persistence of inaccurate beliefs about the self: perseverance effects in the classroom. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  P. Engstrom,et al.  Mammography adherence and psychological distress among women at risk for breast cancer. , 1993, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[14]  S. Folkman [Personal control and stress and coping processes: a theoretical analysis]. , 1988, Kango kenkyu. The Japanese journal of nursing research.

[15]  J. Averill Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to stress. , 1973 .

[16]  M. Stefanek,et al.  Younger women at increased risk for breast cancer: perceived risk, psychological well-being, and surveillance behavior. , 1994, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[17]  S. Nolte,et al.  Adverse psychologic consequences of positive cytologic cervical screening. , 1991, American journal of obstetrics and gynecology.

[18]  A. Bandura Model of Causality in Social Learning Theory , 1985 .

[19]  S. Folkman,et al.  Coping as a mediator of emotion. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  J. Swets Indices of discrimination or diagnostic accuracy: their ROCs and implied models. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  L. Ross,et al.  Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Z. Solomon,et al.  Life-events, coping strategies, social resources, and somatic complaints among combat stress reaction casualties. , 1990, The British journal of medical psychology.

[23]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Effects of components of protection-motivation theory on adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health threat. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  B. Trock,et al.  Psychological side effects of breast cancer screening. , 1991, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[25]  Howard Raiffa,et al.  Decision analysis: introductory lectures on choices under uncertainty. 1968. , 1969, M.D.Computing.

[26]  B. Rimer,et al.  The impact of mailing psychoeducational materials to women with abnormal mammograms. , 1992, American journal of public health.

[27]  Elizabeth H. Creyer,et al.  The influence of firm behavior on purchase intention: do consumers really care about business ethics? , 1997 .

[28]  A. Bandura,et al.  Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. , 1981 .

[29]  J. Sorenson,et al.  Parental response to repeat testing of infants with 'false-positive' results in a newborn screening program. , 1984, Pediatrics.

[30]  B. Rimer,et al.  The impact of a brief coping skills intervention on adherence to breast self‐examination among first‐degree relatives of newly diagnosed breast cancer patients , 1999, Psycho-oncology.

[31]  J. Elmore,et al.  Ten-year risk of false positive screening mammograms and clinical breast examinations. , 1998, The New England journal of medicine.

[32]  Martin F. Davies,et al.  Belief Persistence after Evidential Discrediting: The Impact of Generated versus Provided Explanations on the Likelihood of Discredited Outcomes , 1997 .

[33]  R. Buchanan,et al.  Depression and social stress among patients with benign breast disease , 1986, The British journal of surgery.

[34]  B. Rimer,et al.  A randomized trial of breast cancer risk counseling: the impact on self-reported mammography use. , 1999, American journal of public health.

[35]  G. Bodegård,et al.  Four‐Year Follow‐up of Psychological Reactions to False Positive Screening Tests for Congenital Hypothyroidism , 1987, Acta paediatrica Scandinavica.

[36]  A. Burns The expanded health belief model as a basis for enlightened preventive health care practice and research. , 1992, Journal of health care marketing.

[37]  I. Rosenstock Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model , 1974 .

[38]  S. Thompson Will it hurt less if i can control it? A complex answer to a simple question. , 1981 .

[39]  Philip K. Berger,et al.  A Path Analysis of Preventive Health Care Decision Models , 1979 .

[40]  J. Swets,et al.  A decision-making theory of visual detection. , 1954, Psychological review.

[41]  Chezy Ofir,et al.  Context Effects on Judgment under Uncertainty , 1984 .

[42]  Corporate Sponsorships of Philanthropic Activities: Do They Help the Sponsor? , 2001 .

[43]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change , 1983 .

[44]  M. Bazerman Consumer Research for Consumers , 2001 .

[45]  J. Morris,et al.  Choice of surgery for early breast cancer: psychosocial considerations. , 1988, Social science & medicine.

[46]  S. Folkman,et al.  [An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample]. , 1980, Kango kenkyu. The Japanese journal of nursing research.

[47]  Alan R. Andreasen,et al.  Marketing social change , 1995 .

[48]  C. Carver,et al.  How Important is the Perception of Personal Control? Studies of Early Stage Breast Cancer Patients , 2000 .

[49]  E. Trudeau,et al.  The role of emotions in health care satisfaction. Positive feelings have the expected effect, but negative ones do not always result in dissatisfaction. , 1996, Journal of health care marketing.

[50]  C. Bhattacharya Consumer Reactions to Corporate Social Responsibility , 1999 .

[51]  M. F. Luce,et al.  Choice processing in emotionally difficult decisions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  A. Burns,et al.  The antecedents of preventive health care behavior: An empirical study , 1998 .

[53]  A. Bandura Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[54]  B. Trock,et al.  Psychological and behavioral implications of abnormal mammograms. , 1991, Annals of internal medicine.

[55]  Suzanne M. Miller,et al.  A randomized trial of breast cancer risk counseling: interacting effects of counseling, educational level, and coping style. , 1996, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[56]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Presentation and content: The use of base rates as a continuous variable. , 1988 .

[57]  J. Wardle,et al.  The psychological costs of screening for cancer. , 1992, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[58]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[59]  Use of mammography--United States, 1990. , 1990, MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report.

[60]  M. Bar-Hillel The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. , 1980 .

[61]  Mary Frances Luce,et al.  Avoidance or Vigilance? The Psychology of False-Positive Test Results , 1999 .

[62]  Alan R. Andreasen,et al.  A social marketing research agenda for consumer behavior researchers , 1993 .

[63]  D. Lehmann Market research and analysis , 1979 .

[64]  B. Trock,et al.  Factors associated with repeat adherence to breast cancer screening. , 1990, Preventive medicine.

[65]  I. Gram,et al.  Cancer anxiety and attitudes toward mammography among screening attenders, nonattenders, and women never invited. , 1992, American journal of public health.

[66]  D. Terry,et al.  Appraised controllability as a moderator of the effectiveness of different coping strategies: A test of the goodness of fit hypothesis , 1992 .

[67]  B. Rimer,et al.  Determinants of Mammography Intentions Among Prior Screenees and Nonscreenees1 , 1993 .

[68]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. , 1982 .

[69]  Presidential Address Consumer Behavior As a Social Science , 2001 .

[70]  B. Trock,et al.  Why do some women get regular mammograms? , 1991, American journal of preventive medicine.

[71]  J. Koehler The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, normative, and methodological challenges , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[72]  C. Moorman,et al.  A Model of Consumers' Preventive Health Behaviors: The Role of Health Motivation and Health Ability , 1993 .

[73]  Linda D. Cameron,et al.  Testing a Model of Mammography Intention1 , 1993 .

[74]  Presidential Address Dimensions of Consumer Expertise Or Lack Thereof , 2000 .