From weighing to willing: Approaching a change decision through pre- or postdecisional mentation

Abstract In order to investigate what makes people feel closer to making a change decision, female undergraduates were asked to employ mental exercises on two unresolved personal problems, one being easy to implement (e.g., subscribing to a newspaper) and one being difficult to implement (e.g., breaking up with a boyfriend). In an exhaustive predecisional exercise subjects deliberated on the expectancies and values of making a change decision. Two less exhaustive predecisional exercises required that subjects imagine enjoying the incentives of having made a change decision either in a realistic or fantasy-like manner. In an exhaustive postdecisional exercise subjects had to come up with a plan on how to implement the decision not yet made and were to imagine themselves executing it. Two less exhaustive postdecisional exercises required subjects either to imagine the execution of one single implemental action, or to deliberate solely on various possible action steps. Both the exhaustive pre- and postdecisional exercises were found to be more effective in increasing subjects' perceived proximity to the act of a change decision than the respective nonexhaustive exercises. This effect was not less pronounced for difficult-to-implement problems than for easy-to-implement problems. In both exhaustive cases, the facilitative effect was not mediated by increases in outcome value or outcome expectancy. For the exhaustive postdecisional exercise, however, the effect was mediated by the formation of implemental intents. Results are interpreted in terms of a phase model of action which conceives of decisions as volitional acts that propel the individual from a deliberative state of mind (weighing) to an implemental state of mind (willing).

[1]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Effects of uncertainty and importance on information search in decision making. , 1968 .

[2]  A. Michotte,et al.  Étude expérimentale sur le choix volontaire et ses antécédents immédiats , 1910 .

[3]  Barbara Means,et al.  The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. , 1983 .

[4]  Lee Roy Beach,et al.  Image theory: Principles, goals, and plans in decision making , 1987 .

[5]  Kurt Lewin,et al.  Intention, will and need. , 1951 .

[6]  E. A. Locke Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives , 1968 .

[7]  A. Davidson,et al.  Error patterns in the prediction of fertility behavior. , 1981 .

[8]  L. Beach,et al.  A Contingency Model for the Selection of Decision Strategies , 1978 .

[9]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[10]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Self-Relevant Scenarios as Mediators of Likelihood Estimates and Compliance: Does Imagining Make It So? , 1982 .

[11]  Peter M. Gollwitzer,et al.  Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. , 1989 .

[12]  H. Heckhausen Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model , 1977 .

[13]  W. Edwards Behavioral decision theory. , 1961, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Craig A. Anderson,et al.  Thoughts about Actions: The Effects of Specificity and Availability of Imagined Behavioral Scripts on Expectations about Oneself and Others , 1987 .

[15]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[16]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[17]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Goal setting and task performance: 1969–1980. , 1981 .

[18]  Masanao Toda,et al.  THE DECISION PROCESS: A PERSPECTIVE† , 1976 .

[19]  C. Anderson Imagination and expectation: The effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal influences. , 1983 .

[20]  H. Heckhausen,et al.  Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind , 1987 .

[21]  I. Janis,et al.  Decision Making: A Psychological Analysis of Conflict, Choice, and Commitment , 1977 .

[22]  Daniel W. McAllister,et al.  The Contingency Model for the Selection of Decision Strategies: An Empirical Test. , 1978 .

[23]  Cal D. Stoltenberg,et al.  Self-efficacy expectancy, outcome expectancy, and outcome value: Relative effects on behavioral intentions. , 1986 .

[24]  Jürgen Beckmann,et al.  Deliberative versus implemental states of mind: The issue of impartiality in predecisional and postdecisional information processing , 1987 .

[25]  Jay J.J. Christensen-Szalanski,et al.  A further examination of the selection of problem-solving strategies: The effects of deadlines and analytic aptitudes , 1980 .

[26]  K. Lewin,et al.  Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis , 1926 .

[27]  Melvin Manis,et al.  Can the availability heuristic explain vividness effects , 1986 .

[28]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[29]  L. Beach,et al.  Subjective expected utility and the prediction of birth-planning decisions , 1979 .

[30]  F. W. Irwin,et al.  Value, cost, and information as determiners of decision. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  Terence R. Mitchell,et al.  Image theory: Descriptive sufficiency of a simple rule for the compatibility test , 1988 .