The effects of feature attention on prestimulus cortical activity in the human visual system.

Covert attention affects prestimulus activity in the visual cortex. Although most studies investigating neural mechanisms of attention have focused on the effects of spatial attention, attention can also be directed to particular features. To investigate the spatiotemporal nature of feature attention, we measured subjects' brain activity using magnetoencephalography (MEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while subjects attended to color or motion of a stimulus based on a visual cue, which was presented 1 s before the stimulus onset. We used the hierarchical Bayesian method that allows us to estimate cortical currents with MEG and fMRI data in the order of millimeters and milliseconds. When subjects attended to color, activity within the color-sensitive area (fusiform gyrus) was selectively enhanced within the prestimulus period. By contrast, when subjects attended to motion, activity within the motion-sensitive area (middle temporal gyrus) was selectively enhanced during this period. This effect was not seen in frontal, parietal, and lower visual areas. Additionally, this effect was transient rather than sustained, suggesting that it differs from temporal aspects of spatial attention. These results suggest that, although both spatial and feature attention modulate prestimulus activity within specific visual areas, neural mechanisms underlying these effects might be different.

[1]  Andrea Hasenstaub,et al.  Persistent cortical activity: mechanisms of generation and effects on neuronal excitability. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Reactivation of networks involved in preparatory states. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Theory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[4]  John T. Serences,et al.  Parietal mechanisms of attentional control: locations, features, and objects , 2005 .

[5]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Hierarchical Bayesian estimation for MEG inverse problem , 2004, NeuroImage.

[6]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Jumping the gun: is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Scope and Limits of Top-Down Attention in Unconscious Visual Processing , 2006, Current Biology.

[8]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Neurobiology of Attention , 2005 .

[9]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  MEG response of human V 4 to chromatic visual stimulus , 2001 .

[11]  M. Kawato,et al.  Attentional shifts towards an expected visual target alter the level of alpha-band oscillatory activity in the human calcarine cortex. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Relating Macroscopic Measures of Brain Activity to Fast, Dynamic Neuronal Interactions , 2000, Neural Computation.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship Between Synchronization Among Neuronal Populations and Their Mean Activity Levels , 1999, Neural Computation.

[14]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[15]  R. Yuste,et al.  Attractor dynamics of network UP states in the neocortex , 2003, Nature.

[16]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  J Leon Kenemans,et al.  Spatio-temporal dynamics of top-down control: directing attention to location and/or color as revealed by ERPs and source modeling. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  Alexander M. Harner,et al.  Task-dependent influences of attention on the activation of human primary visual cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Shun-ichi Amari,et al.  Attention modulation of neural tuning through peak and base rate in correlated firing , 2002, Neural Networks.

[20]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Responses of neurons in the parietal and temporal visual pathways during a motion task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[22]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Spatio-temporal analysis of feature-based attention. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Shifting baselines in attention research , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Zeki,et al.  The architecture of the colour centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review * , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  S. Epstein,et al.  Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[28]  J-M Hopf,et al.  Dynamics of feature binding during object-selective attention , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[30]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  F. Tong,et al.  Decoding Seen and Attended Motion Directions from Activity in the Human Visual Cortex , 2006, Current Biology.

[34]  S. Yoshizawa,et al.  A new method for magnetoencephalography: a three-dimensional magnetometer-spatial filter system , 1999, Neuroscience.

[35]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  J. Gallant,et al.  Time Course of Attention Reveals Different Mechanisms for Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in Area V4 , 2005, Neuron.

[37]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  B Giesbrecht,et al.  Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[39]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attention to both space and feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area V4. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Tsunehiro Takeda,et al.  MEG recording from the human ventro-occipital cortex in response to isoluminant color stimulation , 2005, Visual Neuroscience.

[41]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[43]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  O. Andreassen,et al.  Mice Deficient in Cellular Glutathione Peroxidase Show Increased Vulnerability to Malonate, 3-Nitropropionic Acid, and 1-Methyl-4-Phenyl-1,2,5,6-Tetrahydropyridine , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[47]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  David J Heeger,et al.  Neural correlates of sustained spatial attention in human early visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  E. Halgren,et al.  Dynamic Statistical Parametric Mapping Combining fMRI and MEG for High-Resolution Imaging of Cortical Activity , 2000, Neuron.

[51]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[52]  D. McCormick,et al.  Turning on and off recurrent balanced cortical activity , 2003, Nature.

[53]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Feature-based attention in visual cortex , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.