Distraction Sneakers Decrease the Expected Level of Aggression within Groups: A Game‐Theoretic Model

Hawk‐dove games have been extensively used to predict the conditions under which group‐living animals should defend their resources against potential usurpers. Typically, game‐theoretic models on aggression consider that resource defense may entail energetic and injury costs. However, intruders may also take advantage of owners who are busy fighting to sneak access to unguarded resources, imposing thereby an additional cost on the use of the escalated hawk strategy. In this article we modify the two‐strategy hawk‐dove game into a three‐strategy hawk‐dove‐sneaker game that incorporates a distraction‐sneaking tactic, allowing us to explore its consequences on the expected level of aggression within groups. Our model predicts a lower proportion of hawks and hence lower frequencies of aggressive interactions within groups than do previous two‐strategy hawk‐dove games. The extent to which distraction sneakers decrease the frequency of aggression within groups, however, depends on whether they search only for opportunities to join resources uncovered by other group members or for both unchallenged resources and opportunities to usurp.

[1]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Foraging by common ravens in the presence and absence of territory holders: an experimental analysis of social foraging , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  Three‐Player Social Parasitism Games: Implications for Resource Defense and Group Formation , 2002, The American Naturalist.

[3]  T. Caraco,et al.  Social Foraging Theory , 2018 .

[4]  C. Soos,et al.  Operational sex ratio, mediated by synchrony of female arrival, alters the variance of male mating success in Japanese medaka , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  T. Ljungberg,et al.  A test of the sequential assessment game: fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  G. Holton Sociobiology: the new synthesis? , 1977, Newsletter on science, technology & human values.

[7]  M. Gross Sunfish, salmon, and the evolution of alternative reproductive strategies and tactics in fish , 1984 .

[8]  J. M. Smith,et al.  The Logic of Animal Conflict , 1973, Nature.

[9]  S. Östlund-Nilsson Does paternity or paternal investment determine the level of paternal care and does female choice explain egg stealing in the fifteen-spined stickleback? , 2002 .

[10]  J. Hutchings,et al.  Mating success of alternative maturation phenotypes in male Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar , 1988, Oecologia.

[11]  S. Emlen,et al.  Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. , 1977, Science.

[12]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  Aggressive communication in fulmars (Fulmarus glacialis) competing for food , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  S. Foster Group foraging by a coral reef fish: a mechanism for gaining access to defended resources , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  E. Sirot An evolutionarily stable strategy for aggressiveness in feeding groups , 2000 .

[15]  J. Grant Whether or not to defend? The influence of resource distribution , 1993 .

[16]  Lawrence M. Dill,et al.  The Energetics of Feeding Territoriality in Juvenile Coho Salmon (Oncorhynchus Kisutch) , 1985 .

[17]  D. Kramer,et al.  Guarded resources: the effect of intruder number on the tactics and success of defenders and intruders , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  Ian M. Hamilton,et al.  GROUP FORAGING BY A KLEPTOPARASITIC FISH: A STRONG INFERENCE TEST OF SOCIAL FORAGING MODELS , 2003 .

[19]  L. Dill,et al.  The use of territorial gardening versus kleptoparasitism by a subtropical reef fish (Kyphosus cornelii) is influenced by territory defendability , 2003 .

[20]  Colin A. Chapman,et al.  Producers, Scroungers, and Group Foraging , 1991, The American Naturalist.

[21]  J. Hutchings,et al.  Escalation of an asymmetric contest: mortality resulting from mate competition in Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar , 1987 .

[22]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Resource defense in a group-foraging context , 2003 .

[23]  G. Borgia Sexual Competition in Scatophaga Stercoraria: Size- and Density-Related Changes in Male Ability To Capture Females , 1980 .

[24]  H. L. Levitt,et al.  Competitor-to-resource ratio, a general formulation of operational sex ratio, as a predictor of competitive aggression in Japanese medaka (Pisces: Oryziidae) , 2000 .

[25]  M. Gross,et al.  Alternative reproductive strategies and tactics: diversity within sexes. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[26]  M R Gross,et al.  The evolutionarily stable strategy under individual condition and tactic frequency. , 1995, Journal of theoretical biology.