Counterfactual potency.

Counterfactual thoughts typically take the form of implied or explicit if-then statements. We propose that the multiplicative combination of "if likelihood" (the degree to which the antecedent condition of the counterfactual is perceived to be likely) and "then likelihood" (the perceived conditional likelihood of the outcome of the counterfactual, given the antecedent condition) determine the strength and impact of counterfactuals. This construct, termed counterfactual potency, is a reliable predictor of the degree of influence of counterfactual thinking upon judgments of regret, causation, and responsibility. Through 4 studies, we demonstrate the predictive power of this construct in a variety of contexts and show that it plays a causal role in determining the strength of the effects of counterfactual thought. Implications of counterfactual potency as a central factor of counterfactual influence are discussed.

[1]  John V. Petrocelli,et al.  Event detail and confidence in gambling: The role of counterfactual thought reactions , 2010 .

[2]  Richard E. Petty,et al.  PERSUASION: INSIGHTS FROM THE SELF VALIDATION HYPOTHESIS , 2009 .

[3]  J. J. Seta,et al.  Regret: The Roles of Consistency-Fit and Counterfactual Salience , 2008 .

[4]  E. Higgins,et al.  Author ' s personal copy How preferences for eager versus vigilant judgment strategies affect self-serving conclusions , 2008 .

[5]  Justin T. Buckingham,et al.  Culpable Control and Counterfactual Reasoning in the Psychology of Blame , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[6]  N. Roese,et al.  The Functional Theory of Counterfactual Thinking , 2008, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[7]  Barbara A. Spellman,et al.  On the relation between counterfactual and causal reasoning , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[8]  Derek D. Rucker,et al.  Unpacking attitude certainty: attitude clarity and attitude correctness. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Ian Skurnik,et al.  Metacognitive Experiences and the Intricacies of Setting People Straight: Implications for Debiasing and Public Information Campaigns , 2007 .

[10]  R. Byrne,et al.  Spontaneous counterfactual thoughts and causal explanations , 2006 .

[11]  David W. Green,et al.  Mental simulation and argument , 2006 .

[12]  R. Petty,et al.  The Role of MetaCognition in Social Judgment , 2006 .

[13]  M. Zanna,et al.  Establishing a causal chain: why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  D. Mandel,et al.  “What I did” versus “what I might have done”: Effect of factual versus counterfactual thinking on blame, guilt, and shame in prisoners ☆ , 2005 .

[15]  N. Roese,et al.  The mechanics of imagination: Automaticity and control in counterfactual thinking , 2005 .

[16]  Laurence Goldstein,et al.  Paradoxes: Their Roots, Range and Resolution , 2004 .

[17]  Ruth M. J. Byrne,et al.  Counterfactual thinking: The temporal order effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[18]  Michael J. A. Wohl,et al.  Counterfactual Thinking, Blame Assignment, and Well-Being in Rape Victims , 2003 .

[19]  D. Mandel Judgment dissociation theory: an analysis of differences in causal, counterfactual, and covariational reasoning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  Tamiko Azuma,et al.  “Blaming The Victim” Under Memory Load , 2003, Psychological science.

[21]  R. Byrne Mental models and counterfactual thoughts about what might have been , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  N. Schwarz,et al.  The availability heuristic revisited: Ease of recall and content of recall as distinct sources of information. , 2002 .

[23]  Richard E Petty,et al.  Thought confidence as a determinant of persuasion: the self-validation hypothesis. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Ruth M. J. Byrne,et al.  Semifactual “even if” thinking , 2002 .

[25]  R. Byrne,et al.  Counterfactual thinking about controllable events , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[26]  R. Byrne,et al.  Counterfactual thinking about actions and failures to act. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  N. Roese,et al.  Counterfactual Thinking: The Intersection of Affect and Function , 1997 .

[28]  N. Roese Counterfactual thinking. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  L. Reder,et al.  Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory , 1996 .

[30]  L. Sanna,et al.  Antecedents to Spontaneous Counterfactual Thinking: Effects of Expectancy Violation and Outcome Valence , 1996 .

[31]  Nyla R. Branscombe,et al.  Rape and Accident Counterfactuals: Who Might Have Done Otherwise and Would It Have Changed the Outcome?1 , 1996 .

[32]  N. Schwarz,et al.  The availability heuristic revisited : experienced ease of retrieval in mundane frequency estimates , 1995 .

[33]  James M. Olson,et al.  Counterfactual thinking: A critical overview. , 1995 .

[34]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Counterfactual Thought, Regret, and Superstition: How to Avoid Kicking Yourself , 1995 .

[35]  N. Roese,et al.  What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[36]  T. Gilovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: When Less Is More: Counterfactual Thinking and Satisfaction among Olympic Medalists , 2002 .

[37]  K. Markman,et al.  The Mental Simulation of Better and Worse Possible Worlds , 1993 .

[38]  P. Legrenzi,et al.  Event controllability in counterfactual thinking , 1991 .

[39]  F. Strack,et al.  Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. , 1991 .

[40]  Stephen Barker,et al.  Even, still and counterfactuals , 1991 .

[41]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Temporal order and the perceived mutability of events: implications for blame assignment , 1990 .

[42]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  The Role of Counterfactual Thinking in Judgments of Affect , 1990 .

[44]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  The counterfactual fallacy: Confusing what might have been with what ought to have been , 1990 .

[45]  G. Wells,et al.  Mental Simulation of Causality , 1989 .

[46]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[47]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .

[48]  Joel T. Johnson The Knowledge of What Might have Been , 1986 .

[49]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  A. Tversky,et al.  The simulation heuristic , 1982 .

[51]  D. Hofstadter,et al.  Godel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid , 1979 .

[52]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[53]  W. M. Guire 7 – Personality and Attitude Change: An Information-Processing Theory , 1968 .

[54]  N. Goodman Fact, Fiction, and Forecast , 1955 .