Different mechanisms in learning different second languages: Evidence from English speakers learning Chinese and Spanish

Abstract Word reading has been found to be associated with different neural networks in different languages, with greater involvement of the lexical pathway for opaque languages and greater invovlement of the sub‐lexical pathway for transparent langauges. However, we do not know whether this language divergence can be demonstrated in second langauge learners, how learner's metalinguistic ability would modulate the langauge divergence, or whether learning method would interact with the language divergence. In this study, we attempted to answer these questions by comparing brain activations of Chinese and Spanish word reading in native English‐speaking adults who learned Chinese and Spanish over a 2 week period under three learning conditions: phonological, handwriting, and passive viewing. We found that mapping orthography to phonology in Chinese had greater activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) than in Spanish, suggesting greater invovlement of the lexical pathway in opaque langauges. In contrast, Spanish words evoked greater activation in the left superior temporal gyrus (STG) than English, suggesting greater invovlement of the sublexical pathway for transparant languages. Furthermore, brain‐behavior correlation analyses found that higher phonological awareness and rapid naming were associated with greater activation in the bilateral IFG for Chinese and in the bilateral STG for Spanish, suggesting greater language divergence in participants with higher meta‐linguistic awareness. Finally, a significant interaction between the language and learning condition was found in the left STG and middle frontal gyrus (MFG), with greater activation in handwriting learning than viewing learning in the left STG only for Spanish, and greater activation in handwriting learning than phonological learning in the left MFG only for Chinese. These findings suggest that handwriting facilitates assembled phonology in Spanish and addressed phonology in Chinese. In summary, our study suggests different mechanisms in learning different L2s, providing important insights into neural plasticity and important implications in second language education.

[1]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  Longitudinal predictors of very early Chinese literacy acquisition , 2011 .

[2]  H. Cheung,et al.  Metalinguistic skills and vocabulary knowledge in Chinese (L1) and English (L2) , 2006 .

[3]  Jean-Luc Velay,et al.  Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing , 2003, NeuroImage.

[4]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological Skills and Learning to Read , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[5]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition: cross-language and writing system transfer , 2005, Cognition.

[7]  T. Shanahan,et al.  Phonemic Awareness Instruction Helps Children Learn to Read: Evidence From the National Reading Panel's Meta-Analysis , 2001 .

[8]  L. Katz,et al.  Cerebral organization of component processes in reading. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  P. Matthews,et al.  Testing for Dual Brain Processing Routes in Reading: A Direct Contrast of Chinese Character and Pinyin Reading Using fMRI , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  P. Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[11]  L. Tan,et al.  Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  F. Ramus,et al.  Exploring dyslexics' phonological deficit I: lexical vs sub-lexical and input vs output processes. , 2005, Dyslexia.

[13]  Octavian Iordache,et al.  Assimilation and Accommodation , 2018, Lecture Notes in Intelligent Transportation and Infrastructure.

[14]  Lindsay N. Harris,et al.  Writing affects the brain network of reading in Chinese: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[15]  Chun Qi Chang,et al.  China’s language input system in the digital age affects children’s reading development , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  C. Brennan,et al.  The brain adapts to orthography with experience: evidence from English and Chinese. , 2015, Developmental science.

[17]  James R. Booth,et al.  Development of brain networks involved in spoken word processing of Mandarin Chinese , 2011, NeuroImage.

[18]  James R. Booth,et al.  Cultural Constraints on Brain Development: Evidence from a Developmental Study of Visual Word Processing in Mandarin Chinese , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  The role of rapid naming in reading development and dyslexia in Chinese. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[20]  Viorica Marian,et al.  The Effect of Second-Language Experience on Native-Language Processing. , 2011, Vigo international journal of applied linguistics.

[21]  Xu Yi,et al.  The Effect of Radical-Based Grouping in Character Learning in Chinese as a Foreign Language , 2014 .

[22]  D. Funder Global Traits: A Neo-Allportian Approach to Personality , 1991 .

[23]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[24]  Eileen M. Napoliello,et al.  Neural basis of single‐word reading in Spanish–English bilinguals , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[25]  P T Fox,et al.  An fMRI study with written Chinese , 2001, Neuroreport.

[26]  James R. Booth,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Intra- and Cross-Modal Lexical Tasks , 2002, NeuroImage.

[27]  María S. Carlo,et al.  Bilingual phonological awareness: Multilevel construct validation among Spanish-speaking kindergarteners in transitional bilingual education classrooms , 2006 .

[28]  S. Petersen,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of word reading. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  Conrad Perry,et al.  The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German , 2000 .

[30]  L. Katz,et al.  Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). , 2000, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.

[31]  Richard K. Olson,et al.  Phonological Deficits, IQ, and Individual Differences in Reading Disability: Genetic and Environmental Influences , 1993 .

[32]  Uttam Kumar,et al.  Effect of orthography over neural regions in bilinguals: A view from neuroimaging , 2014, Neuroscience Letters.

[33]  Charles A. P Erfetti,et al.  Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain's reading network , 2007, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[34]  D. Bishop,et al.  A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. , 1990, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[35]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Beyond the word and image: characteristics of a common meaning system for language and vision revealed by functional and structural imaging , 2015, NeuroImage.

[36]  Kaitlyn A. Litcofsky,et al.  Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain , 2014, Cortex.

[37]  K. James,et al.  The effects of handwriting experience on functional brain development in pre-literate children , 2012, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[38]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  Bilingualism, Language Proficiency, and Learning to Read in Two Writing Systems , 2005 .

[39]  I. Kanazawa,et al.  Different cortical activity in reading of Kanji words, Kana words and Kana nonwords. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[40]  J. Velay,et al.  The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[41]  E. Marinus,et al.  Phonics training for English-speaking poor readers. , 2018, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[42]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Implicit and explicit processing of kanji and kana words and non-words studied with fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[43]  Usha Goswami,et al.  Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective , 2002 .

[44]  P. Fox,et al.  The Neural System Underlying Chinese Logograph Reading , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  Syllable, Phoneme, and Tone: Psycholinguistic Units in Early Chinese and English Word Recognition , 2008 .

[46]  James R. Booth,et al.  Reading acquisition reorganizes the phonological awareness network only in alphabetic writing systems , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Connie Qun Guan,et al.  Writing strengthens orthography and alphabetic-coding strengthens phonology in learning to read Chinese , 2011 .

[48]  P. F. Jong,et al.  Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. , 2003 .

[49]  Fan Cao,et al.  How does language distance between L1 and L2 affect the L2 brain network? An fMRI study of Korean–Chinese–English trilinguals , 2016, NeuroImage.

[50]  Jin Tian,et al.  Eye movements of second language learners when reading spaced and unspaced Chinese texts , 2012, Frontiers in Psychology.

[51]  J. Fiez,et al.  Assimilation and accommodation patterns in ventral occipitotemporal cortex in learning a second writing system , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[52]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST HOW PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE INFORMS THE TEACHING OF READING , 2022 .

[53]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[54]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  See Dick RAN: Rapid Naming and the Longitudinal Prediction of Reading Subskills in First and Second Graders , 1999 .

[55]  Kim A. Lindsey,et al.  Prediction of First-Grade Reading in Spanish-Speaking English-Language Learners. , 2003 .

[56]  K. James,et al.  The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: Tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[57]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[58]  Julia M. Carroll,et al.  Language and phonological skills in children at high risk of reading difficulties. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[59]  Ying Liu,et al.  The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.

[60]  C. McBride-Chang,et al.  Morphological awareness and early and advanced word recognition and spelling in Dutch , 2008 .

[61]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Segregating Semantic from Phonological Processes during Reading , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.  Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing–Second Edition , 2016 .

[63]  Gui Xue,et al.  Native language experience shapes neural basis of addressed and assembled phonologies , 2015, NeuroImage.

[64]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Letter processing in the visual system: Different activation patterns for single letters and strings , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[65]  Karin H. James,et al.  Brain activation patterns resulting from learning letter forms through active self-production and passive observation in young children , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[66]  Nadia E. Cone,et al.  Neural correlates of orthographic and phonological consistency effects in children , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[67]  K. James Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. , 2010, Developmental science.

[68]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[69]  G. Xue,et al.  Artificial Language Training Reveals the Neural Substrates Underlying Addressed and Assembled Phonologies , 2014, PloS one.

[70]  Marieke Longcamp,et al.  The imprint of action: Motor cortex involvement in visual perception of handwritten letters , 2006, NeuroImage.

[71]  Jay G. Rueckl,et al.  Neural division of labor in reading is constrained by culture: A training study of reading Chinese characters , 2014, Cortex.

[72]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  Rapid naming speed components and reading development in a consistent orthography. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[73]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[74]  J. Xiong,et al.  Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language , 2003 .

[75]  Stephen R. Burgess,et al.  Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts , 2005 .

[76]  Naama Friedmann,et al.  Rapid language-related plasticity: microstructural changes in the cortex after a short session of new word learning , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.

[77]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Early stage visual-orthographic processes predict long-term retention of word form and meaning: A visual encoding training study , 2013, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[78]  W. Nagy,et al.  Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. , 1993 .

[79]  Cynthia A. Riccio,et al.  WHAT DO WE KNOW ABOUT PHONOLOGICAL AWARENESS IN SPANISH , 2000 .

[80]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[81]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[82]  E. Marinus,et al.  Phonics training for English-speaking poor readers. , 2012, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[83]  Jean-Claude Gilhodes,et al.  Learning through Hand- or Typewriting Influences Visual Recognition of New Graphic Shapes: Behavioral and Functional Imaging Evidence , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  C. Leong Developmental dyslexia as developmental and linguistic variation: Editor’s commentary , 2002 .

[85]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[86]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  What Is in the Naming? A 5-Year Longitudinal Study of Early Rapid Naming and Phonological Sensitivity in Relation to Subsequent Reading Skills in Both Native Chinese and English as a Second Language , 2011 .

[87]  Zhen Jin,et al.  Developmental differences of neurocognitive networks for phonological and semantic processing in Chinese word reading , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[88]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[89]  Fan Cao,et al.  Specialization and Universals in the Development of Reading Skill: How Chinese Research Informs a Universal Science of Reading , 2013, Scientific studies of reading : the official journal of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading.