Beware and be aware: Capture of spatial attention by fear-related stimuli in neglect

Stimuli with threat significance may be privileged in summoning attention, allowing fast detection even outside the field of attention. We studied patients with unilateral neglect and visual extinction, who usually remain unaware of contralesional stimuli presented together with concurrent ipsilesional stimuli, to learn whether emotional stimuli might differentially be affected by contralesional extinction. Pictures of spiders or flowers with similar features were presented in right, left, or both fields. On bilateral trials, the patients detected emotional stimuli (spiders) on the left side much more often than neutral pictures (flowers). While mechanisms of spatial attention are impaired after parietal damage in neglect patients, intact visual pathways to the ventral temporal lobe and amygdala might still mediate distinct mechanisms of emotional attention.

[1]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional facial expressions capture attention , 2001, Neurology.

[2]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[3]  D. Purcell,et al.  It Takes a Confounded Face to Pop Out of a Crowd , 1996, Perception.

[4]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. , 1995 .

[5]  M. Kindt,et al.  Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the stroop task. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  A. Ohman,et al.  Fear appears fast: temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[7]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  Faces call for attention: evidence from patients with visual extinction , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  K. Mogg,et al.  ATTENTIONAL BIASES FOR EMOTIONAL FACES , 1997 .

[11]  Giacomo Rizzolatti,et al.  Visual Processing without Awareness: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[13]  S. Stone-Elander,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of visually elicited simple phobic fear: additional data and theoretical analysis. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[15]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  A. Mack Inattentional Blindness , 2003 .

[17]  J. Eccles The emotional brain. , 1980, Bulletin et memoires de l'Academie royale de medecine de Belgique.

[18]  H. Merckelbach,et al.  Phobic dimensions: III. factor analytic approaches to the study of common phobic fears; An updated review of findings obtained with adult subjects , 1991 .

[19]  A. Öhman Face the beast and fear the face: animal and social fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. , 1986 .

[20]  Manjit,et al.  Neurology , 1912, NeuroImage.

[21]  R. Dolan,et al.  Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Reply: Vision and attention: the role of training , 1998, Nature.

[23]  J. Armfield,et al.  Vulnerability representation: the role of perceived dangerousness, uncontrollability, unpredictability and disgustingness in spider fear. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[24]  R. Rafal,et al.  A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.