Willed action and attention to the selection of action

Actions are said to be 'willed' if we consciously pay attention to their selection. It has been suggested that they are associated with activations in the dorsal prefrontal cortex (area 46). However, because previous experiments typically used a 'free selection' paradigm to examine this hypothesis, it is unclear whether the results reflected the attention to the selection of action or the freedom of choice allowed by the tasks. In this experiment, we minimized the difference of working memory demand across task conditions by using novel stimuli in each trial. We found that activation in the dorsal prefrontal cortex on a free selection task was not significantly different from that induced by another task that required attention to the selection of action, although the responses were externally specified. This suggests that the dorsal prefrontal cortex is in fact associated with attention to the selection of action, but does not play a unique role in the generation of internally initiated actions. However, the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) may subserve this function as activity in this region was found to be tightly associated with the free selection of responses.

[1]  C. D. Frith,et al.  The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Random Number Generation: A Study with Positron Emission Tomography , 2000, NeuroImage.

[2]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  S. Petersen,et al.  Characterizing the Hemodynamic Response: Effects of Presentation Rate, Sampling Procedure, and the Possibility of Ordering Brain Activity Based on Relative Timing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[4]  E. Phelps,et al.  "Willed action": a functional MRI study of the human prefrontal cortex during a sensorimotor task. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and attention to action , 2005 .

[6]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  B Conrad,et al.  Time-resolved fMRI of activation patterns in M1 and SMA during complex voluntary movement. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  C. D. Frith,et al.  The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Evidence from the Effects of Contextual Constraint in a Sentence Completion Task , 2002, NeuroImage.

[9]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Distribution of readiness potential, pre-motion positivity, and motor potential of the human cerebral cortex preceding voluntary finger movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  P F Liddle,et al.  The role of the left prefrontal cortex in verbal processing: semantic processing or willed action? , 1994, Neuroreport.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[13]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  B. Postle,et al.  Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  P. Goldman-Rakic Architecture of the Prefrontal Cortex and the Central Executive , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  S A Spence,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activity in people with schizophrenia and control subjects , 1998, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Benjamin Libet,et al.  The Volitional Brain: Towards a Neuroscience of Free Will , 1999 .

[19]  L. Deecke Bereitschaftspotential as an indicator of movement preparation in supplementary motor area and motor cortex. , 1987, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[20]  A. Berthoz,et al.  An anatomical landmark for the supplementary eye fields in human revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[22]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[24]  M Gangitano,et al.  Chronometry of parietal and prefrontal activations in verbal working memory revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2003, NeuroImage.

[25]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Interference with Performance of a Response Selection Task that has no Working Memory Component: An rTMS Comparison of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Medial Frontal Cortex , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus–response compatibility on brain activation , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[28]  R. Buckner,et al.  Dissociation of human prefrontal cortical areas across different speech production tasks and gender groups. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  P. Goldman-Rakic Working memory and the mind. , 1992, Scientific American.

[30]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[31]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[35]  C. Frith,et al.  Towards a functional anatomy of volition , 1999 .

[36]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Willed action and its impairments. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[38]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cortical areas and the selection of movement: a study with positron emission tomography , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Albert Gjedde,et al.  Origin of Human Motor Readiness Field Linked to Left Middle Frontal Gyrus by MEG and PET , 1998, NeuroImage.

[41]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[42]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The mode of movement selection Movement-related cortical potentials prior to freely selected and repetitive movements , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  M. Petrides The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[45]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who's in control? , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  R. Passingham,et al.  The functions of the medial premotor cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[48]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Impaired mesial frontal and putamen activation in Parkinson's disease: A positron emission tomography study , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[49]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  C. Frith,et al.  The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the selection of action as revealed by functional imaging , 2000 .

[51]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Sustained Activity in the Medial Wall during Working Memory Delays , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Masud Seyal,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of left prefrontal cortex impairs working memory , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[53]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[54]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[55]  J. Desmond,et al.  Dissociation of Frontal and Cerebellar Activity in a Cognitive Task: Evidence for a Distinction between Selection and Search , 1998, NeuroImage.