The direction of retinal motion facilitates binocular steropsis

Visual information from binocular disparity and from relative motion provide information about three–dimensional structure and layout of the world. Although the mechanisms that process these cues have typically been studied independently, there is now a substantial body of evidence that suggests that they interact in the visual pathway. This paper investigates one advantage of such an interaction: whether retinal motion can be used as a matching constraint in the binocular correspondence process. Stimuli that contained identical disparity and motion signals but which differed in their fine–scale correlation were created to establish whether the direction, or the speed, of motion could enhance performance in a psychophysical task in which binocular matching is a limiting factor. The results of these experiments provide clear evidence that different directions of motion, but not different speeds, are processed separately in stereopsis. The results fit well with properties of neurons early in the cortical visual pathway which are thought to be involved in determining local matches between features in the two eyes' images.

[1]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Eric Castet,et al.  Stereoscopic depth perception at high velocities , 1996 .

[3]  Local vs global stereoscopic matching in neurons of cortical area V1 , 1996 .

[4]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues , 1995, Vision Research.

[5]  C. WILLIAM TYLER,et al.  Depth perception in disparity gratings , 1974, Nature.

[6]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Disparity sensitivity of neurons in monkey extrastriate area MST , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  S. McKee,et al.  Steroscopic acuity for moving retinal images. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[8]  Yukie Saito,et al.  Integration of Stereopsis and Motion Shape Cues , 1993 .

[9]  R. Andersen,et al.  Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques , 1995, Nature.

[10]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Stereo-motion cooperation and the use ofmotion disparity in the visual perception of 3-D structure , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  S M Anstis,et al.  Movement Aftereffects Contingent on Binocular Disparity , 1974, Perception.

[12]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.

[13]  B. Rogers,et al.  The Interaction of Binocular Disparity and Motion Parallax in the Computation of Depth , 1996, Vision Research.

[14]  Ting-Chuen Pong,et al.  Stereo and Motion Cues in Preattentive Vision Processing—Some Experiments with Random-Dot Stereographic Image Sequences , 1990 .

[15]  J S Tittle,et al.  Recovery of 3-D shape from binocular disparity and structure from motion , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  D. Regan,et al.  Cyclopean Discrimination Thresholds for the Direction and Speed of Motion in Depth , 1996, Vision Research.

[17]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[18]  Christopher W. Tyler,et al.  Sensory processing of binocular disparity , 1983 .

[19]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Independent neural mechanisms for bright and dark information in binocular stereopsis , 1995, Nature.

[20]  Julie M. Harris Early vision and beyond , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  A. Parker,et al.  Spatial properties of disparity pooling in human stereo vision , 1989, Vision Research.

[22]  J T Todd,et al.  The perception of stereoscopic transparency , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  M Nawrot,et al.  Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. , 1989, Science.

[24]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Computational Issues in Solving the Stereo Correspondence ProblemComputational Issues in Solving the Stereo Correspondence Problem , 1991 .

[27]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Kenneth J. Ciuffreda,et al.  Vergence eye movements : basic and clinical aspects , 1983 .

[29]  R. Blake,et al.  The interplay between stereopsis and structure from motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth , 1994, Vision Research.

[31]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  C W Tyler,et al.  Mechanisms for dynamic stereomotion respond selectively to horizontal velocity components , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. , 1990, Science.

[34]  G. Orban,et al.  Response to movement of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat: velocity sensitivity. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  T. Papathomas Early vision and beyond , 1995 .

[36]  G. Poggio,et al.  Mechanisms of static and dynamic stereopsis in foveal cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[37]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Psychophysical isolation of movement sensitivity by removal of familiar position cues , 1981, Vision Research.