Storage for free: a surprising property of a simple gain-control model of motion aftereffects

If a motion aftereffect (MAE) for given adaptation conditions has a duration T s, and the eyes are closed after adaptation during a waiting period tw=T s before testing, an unexpected MAE of a 'residual' duration TrT s is experienced. This effect is called 'storage' and it is often quantified by a storage factor sigma=TrT/T, which can reach values up to about 0.7-0.8. The phenomenon and its name have invited explanations in terms of inhibition of recovery during darkness. We present a model based on the opposite idea, that an effective test stimulus quickens recovery relative to darkness or other ineffective test stimuli. The model is worked out in mathematical detail and proves to explain 'storage' data from the literature, on the static MAE (sMAE: an MAE experienced for static test stimuli). We also present results of a psychophysical experiment with moving random pixel arrays, quantifying storage phenomena both for the sMAE and the dynamic MAE (dMAE: an MAE experienced for a random dynamic noise test stimulus). Storage factors for the dMAE are lower than for the sMAE. Our model also gives an excellent description of these new data on storage of the dMAE. The term 'storage' might therefore be a misnomer. If an effective test stimulus influences all direction tuned motion sensors indiscriminately and thus speeds up equalization of gains, one gets the storage phenomenon for free.

[1]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Adapting coincidence scalers and neural modelling studies of vision , 1971, Kybernetik.

[2]  N J Wade,et al.  A Selective History of the Study of Visual Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.

[3]  I. M. Spigel The Effects of Differential Post-Exposure Illumination on the Decay of a Movement After-Effect , 1960 .

[4]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  A model of transparent motion and non-transparent motion aftereffects , 1995, NIPS.

[5]  Irwin M. Spigel,et al.  Relation of movement aftereffect duration to interpolated darkness intervals , 1962 .

[6]  James E. McCarthy Directional adaptation effects with contrast modulated stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.

[7]  N Osaka,et al.  Difference of Spatial Frequency Selectivity between Static and Flicker Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.

[8]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Movement aftereffect of bi-vectorial transparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[9]  G. Mather The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.

[10]  N. Sutherland Figural After-Effects and Apparent Size , 1961 .

[11]  I. M. Spigel THE USE OF DECAY INHIBITION IN AN EXAMINATION OF CENTRAL MEDIATION IN MOVEMENT AFTEREFFECTS. , 1964, The Journal of general psychology.

[12]  H. Barlow,et al.  Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.

[13]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  A new transparent motion aftereffect , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  B Moulden,et al.  In Defence of a Ratio Model for Movement Detection at Threshold , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Slow and fast visual motion channels have independent binocular–rivalry stages , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Aftereffect of High-Speed Motion , 1998, Perception.

[17]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Perception of Movement and Correlation in Stroboscopically Presented Noise Patterns , 1985, Perception.

[18]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[19]  S. Exner,et al.  Entwurf zu einer physiologischen Erklärung der psychischen Erscheinungen , 1894 .

[20]  Michael von Grünau,et al.  A motion aftereffect for long-range troboscopic apparent motion , 1986 .

[21]  M. Carandini,et al.  A tonic hyperpolarization underlying contrast adaptation in cat visual cortex. , 1997, Science.

[22]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing , 1996, Nature.

[23]  T. Ledgeway Adaptation to second-order motion results in a motion aftereffect for directionally-ambiguous test stimuli , 1994, Vision Research.

[24]  M. V. von Grünau A motion aftereffect for long-range stroboscopic apparent motion. , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Membrane Mechanisms Underlying Contrast Adaptation in Cat Area 17In Vivo , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[27]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Limits in perception , 1984 .

[28]  J. Movshon,et al.  Linearity and Normalization in Simple Cells of the Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The motion aftereffect , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  A. Pantle,et al.  Motion aftereffect as a function of the contrast of sinusoidal gratings , 1976, Vision Research.

[31]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Aftereffect of Seen Motion with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1963, Science.

[32]  B. C. Griffith,et al.  Some Observations on the Spiral After-Effect , 1959 .

[33]  Naoyuki Osaka,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test stimuli depends on adapting velocity , 1995, Vision Research.

[34]  R. Blake,et al.  Another means for measuring the motion aftereffect , 1993, Vision Research.

[35]  M. J. Keck,et al.  Recovery from Adaptation to Moving Gratings , 1977, Perception.

[36]  J. Hulleman,et al.  Gain control and hyperpolarization level in cat horizontal cells as a function of light and dark adaptation , 1996, Vision Research.

[37]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Temporal Transfer Properties of the Afferent Visual System Psychophysical,Neurophysiological and Theoretical Investigations , 1973 .

[38]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Models of the processing of quantum signals by the human peripheral retina , 1970, Kybernetik.

[39]  I. M. Spigel Contour absence as a critical factor in the inhibition of the decay of a movement aftereffect. , 1962 .

[40]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[41]  M. Lankheet,et al.  A gain-control model relating nulling results to the duration of dynamic motion aftereffects , 2003, Vision Research.

[42]  P. Thompson,et al.  The Role of Intervening Patterns in the Storage of the Movement Aftereffect , 1994, Perception.

[43]  A Pantle Adaptation to pattern spatial frequency: effects on visual movement sensitivity in humans. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[44]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Neural correlates of motion after-effects in cat striate cortical neurones: monocular adaptation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Recovery from motion adaptation is delayed by successively presented orthogonal motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[46]  R Blake,et al.  Another perspective on the visual motion aftereffect. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  J. Movshon,et al.  Neuronal Adaptation to Visual Motion in Area MT of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.

[48]  Stephen T. Hammett,et al.  The dynamics of velocity adaptation in human vision , 2000, Current Biology.

[49]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Rapid motion aftereffect seen within uniform flickering test fields , 1983, Nature.

[50]  A Pantle,et al.  A model for after-effects of seen movement. , 1967, Vision research.

[51]  G. Mather,et al.  Evidence for second-order motion detectors , 1993, Vision Research.