Lapses of Intention and Performance Variability Reveal Age-Related Increases in Fluctuations of Executive Control

We examine the hypothesis that the efficiency of executive control processes is less stable over time in older than younger adults. An age-related decrease in the efficiency of executive control should result in an increase in performance variability in task conditions requiring the recruitment of executive control processes and not in task conditions requiring minimal involvement of executive control. Performance variability was similar for younger and older adults in task conditions requiring minimal executive control and greater for older than younger adults in task conditions requiring executive control. These and other data are consistent with the proposal that aging is associated with a decrease in the stability of executive control over time.

[1]  H. Levene Robust tests for equality of variances , 1961 .

[2]  Morton B. Brown,et al.  Robust Tests for the Equality of Variances , 1974 .

[3]  L. Poon,et al.  New directions in memory and aging : proceedings of the George A. Talland Memorial Conference , 1980 .

[4]  T. Triffet,et al.  Aspects of Consciousness , 1997 .

[5]  Jeff Miller,et al.  A warning about median reaction time , 1988 .

[6]  J. Myerson,et al.  Age, variability, and speed: between-subjects diversity. , 1988, Psychology and aging.

[7]  D. Stuss,et al.  Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  D. Mewhort,et al.  Analysis of Response Time Distributions: An Example Using the Stroop Task , 1991 .

[10]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[11]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Optimal Time of Day and the Magnitude of Age Differences in Memory , 1993 .

[12]  T. Salthouse Attentional blocks are not responsible for age-related slowing. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.

[13]  Claire Morse,et al.  Does variability increase with age? An archival study of cognitive measures. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[14]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Age, Information Processing Speed, and Intelligence , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  A. Mackinnon,et al.  Age differences and interindividual variation in cognition in community-dwelling elderly. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[16]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Characterization of stability of performance in patients with traumatic brain injury: Variability and consistency on reaction time tests. , 1994 .

[17]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  E. Maylor,et al.  Age-related impairment in an event-based prospective-memory task. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[19]  D. Balota,et al.  Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J. Duncan,et al.  Intelligence and the Frontal Lobe: The Organization of Goal-Directed Behavior , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  P. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[22]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  D. Head,et al.  Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Aging and Variability in Performance , 1998 .

[25]  L Hasher,et al.  Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  The complex nature of unique and shared effects in hierarchical linear regression : Implications for developmental psychology , 1998 .

[27]  Sergio Della Sala,et al.  Aging, intelligence, and anatomical segregation in the frontal lobes , 1998 .

[28]  H. Markowitsch Cognitive neuroscience of memory , 1998 .

[29]  T. Cutmore,et al.  Performance of individuals with severe long-term traumatic brain injury on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[30]  Siu Cheung Li,et al.  Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotrans , 1999 .

[31]  R. West Age differences in lapses of intention in the Stroop task. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[32]  F. Craik,et al.  Age-related decline in prospective memory: the roles of cue accessibility and cue sensitivity. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[33]  R. West,et al.  Visual distraction, working memory, and aging , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[34]  R. West,et al.  Evidence for the transient nature of a neural system supporting goal-directed action. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  P. Greenwood,et al.  The frontal aging hypothesis evaluated , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[36]  R. West,et al.  Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task , 2000, Brain Research.

[37]  D. Spieler,et al.  Modelling age-related changes in information processing , 2001 .

[38]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of cognitive aging , 2001 .

[39]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition , 2001 .

[40]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  There are stable individual differences in performance variability, both from moment to moment and from day to day , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.