Altered perception-action binding modulates inhibitory control in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome.

BACKGROUND Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome (GTS) is a multifaceted neuropsychiatric developmental disorder with onset in childhood or adolescence and frequent remissions in early adulthood. A rather new emerging concept of this syndrome suggests that it is a disorder of purposeful actions, in which sensory processes and their relation to motor responses (actions) play a particularly important role. Thus, this syndrome might be conceived as a condition of altered 'perception-action binding'. In the current study, we test this novel concept in the context of inhibitory control. METHODS We examined N = 35 adolescent GTS patients and N = 39 healthy controls in a Go/Nogo-task manipulating the complexity of sensory information triggering identical actions; i.e. to inhibit a motor response. This was combined with event-related potential recordings, EEG data decomposition and source localization. RESULTS GTS patients showed worse performance compared to controls and larger performance differences when inhibitory control had to be exerted using unimodal visual compared to bimodal auditory-visual stimuli. This suggests increased binding between bimodal stimuli and responses leading to increased costs of switching between responses instructed by bimodal and those instructed by unimodal stimuli. The neurophysiological data showed that this was related to mechanisms mediating between stimulus evaluation and response selection; i.e. perception-action binding processes in the right inferior parietal cortex (BA40). CONCLUSIONS Stimulus-action inhibition binding is stronger in GTS patients than healthy controls and affects inhibitory control corroborating the concept suggesting that GTS might be a condition of altered perception-action integration (binding); i.e. a disorder of purposeful actions.

[1]  S. Lehéricy,et al.  Neuroimaging of tic genesis: Present status and future perspectives , 2015, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[2]  Andrea Hildebrandt,et al.  Exploiting the intra-subject latency variability from single-trial event-related potentials in the P3 time range: A review and comparative evaluation of methods , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  C. Beste,et al.  Distinguishing stimulus and response codes in theta oscillations in prefrontal areas during inhibitory control of automated responses , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[4]  E Bricolo,et al.  Redundancy gain in the stop-signal paradigm: implications for the locus of coactivation in simple reaction time. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[6]  O. Fründt,et al.  Behavioral therapy for Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorders. , 2017, Neurology. Clinical practice.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Go/NoGo Performance in Boys with Tourette Syndrome , 2010, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[9]  M. Greenlee,et al.  Redundancy gains in simple responses and go/no-go tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Verena C. Seibold,et al.  The influence of dichotical fusion on the redundant signals effect, localization performance, and the mismatch negativity , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  C. Beste,et al.  Concurrent information affects response inhibition processes via the modulation of theta oscillations in cognitive control networks , 2015, Brain Structure and Function.

[13]  S. Channon,et al.  Tourette's syndrome (TS): cognitive performance in adults with uncomplicated TS. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[14]  L. Baer,et al.  Habit reversal versus supportive psychotherapy for Tourette's disorder: a randomized controlled trial. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[15]  B. Hommel,et al.  Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration , 2004 .

[16]  Christian Beste,et al.  The norepinephrine system shows information-content specific properties during cognitive control – Evidence from EEG and pupillary responses , 2017, NeuroImage.

[17]  G. Jackson,et al.  Enhanced Cognitive Control in Young People with Tourette's Syndrome , 2006, Current Biology.

[18]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  On the role of fronto-striatal neural synchronization processes for response inhibition—Evidence from ERP phase-synchronization analyses in pre-manifest Huntington's disease gene mutation carriers , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[20]  B. Hommel Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding) , 2009, Psychological research.

[21]  C. Beste,et al.  Altered perceptual binding in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome , 2016, Cortex.

[22]  P. Haggard,et al.  Action inhibition in Tourette syndrome , 2014, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[23]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[24]  J L Bradshaw,et al.  The Simon effect and attention deficits in Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and Huntington's disease. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  Costs of control: decreased motor cortex engagement during a Go/NoGo task in Tourette's syndrome. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  Christian Beste,et al.  Neuronal Intra-Individual Variability Masks Response Selection Differences between ADHD Subtypes—A Need to Change Perspectives , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  B. Hommel,et al.  Moderate alcohol consumption in humans impairs feature binding in visual perception but not across perception and action , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[28]  W. Sommer,et al.  Residue iteration decomposition (RIDE): A new method to separate ERP components on the basis of latency variability in single trials. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[29]  J. Baudewig,et al.  Normal response inhibition in boys with Tourette syndrome , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[30]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  The functional anatomy of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  Christina F. Lavallee,et al.  Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[32]  C. Beste,et al.  Tics and Tourette syndrome — surplus of actions rather than disorder? , 2018, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[33]  Werner Sommer,et al.  A toolbox for residue iteration decomposition (RIDE)—A method for the decomposition, reconstruction, and single trial analysis of event related potentials , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[34]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  Evidence for enhanced multi-component behaviour in Tourette syndrome – an EEG study , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[35]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Testing the stimulus-to-response bridging function of the oddball-P3 by delayed response signals and residue iteration decomposition (RIDE) , 2014, NeuroImage.

[36]  Christian Beste,et al.  Response mode-dependent differences in neurofunctional networks during response inhibition: an EEG-beamforming study , 2016, Brain Structure and Function.

[37]  C. Whittington,et al.  Practitioner Review: Treatments for Tourette syndrome in children and young people - a systematic review. , 2016, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced habit formation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. , 2016, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Christian Beste,et al.  Neural mechanisms and functional neuroanatomical networks during memory and cue-based task switching as revealed by residue iteration decomposition (RIDE) based source localization , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.

[40]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  P. Haggard,et al.  The neural correlates of tic inhibition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  V. Roessner,et al.  Effects of multisensory integration processes on response inhibition in adolescent autism spectrum disorder , 2016, Psychological Medicine.

[43]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Overcoming limitations of the ERP method with Residue Iteration Decomposition (RIDE): a demonstration in go/no-go experiments. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 2001 .

[45]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  A systems neurophysiology approach to voluntary event coding , 2016, NeuroImage.

[46]  E. Walker,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[47]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Review Re-evaluating the Role of Tpj in Attentional Control: Contextual Updating? , 2022 .