Extending "out of the body" saltation to 2d mobile tactile interaction

Funneling and saltation are two main perceptual illusion techniques for vibro-tactile feedback. They are often used to minimize the number of vibrators to be worn on the body and thereby build a less cumbersome and expensive feedback device. Recently, these techniques have been found to elicit "out of the body" experience, i.e. feeling for phantom sensations indirectly on a hand-held object. This paper explores the practical applicability of this theoretical result to mobile tactile interaction. Two psychophysical experiments were run to validate: (1) the 1D saltation effect through the hand-held smart phone, and (2) the effect of saltation based approach to 2D phantom sensation elicitation. Experimental results have first confirmed the same "out of the body" saltation effect in 1D, originally tested on a metallic ruler by Miyazaki [15], on an actual mobile device. In addition, 2D modulated phantom sensation with a resolution of 5 x 3 on a 3.5 inch display space was achieved with saltation based stimulation.

[1]  R W Cholewiak,et al.  The generation of vibrotactile patterns on a linear array: Influences of body site, time, and presentation mode , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  D. S. Alles,et al.  Information Transmission by Phantom Sensations , 1970 .

[3]  Roope Raisamo,et al.  Evaluating the effect of temporal parameters for vibrotactile saltatory patterns , 2009, ICMI-MLMI '09.

[4]  Seungmoon Choi,et al.  Initial study for creating linearly moving vibrotactile sensation on mobile device , 2010, 2010 IEEE Haptics Symposium.

[5]  G. Békésy,et al.  Funneling in the Nervous System and its Role in Loudness and Sensation Intensity on the Skin , 1958 .

[6]  Abdulmotaleb El Saddik,et al.  Spatial resolution of vibrotactile perception on the human forearm when exploiting funneling illusion , 2009, 2009 IEEE International Workshop on Haptic Audio visual Environments and Games.

[7]  N. Prins Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .

[8]  Alina Hang,et al.  The PhantomStation: towards funneling remote tactile feedback on interactive surfaces , 2011, AH '11.

[9]  Alex Pentland,et al.  Tactual displays for wearable computing , 2005, Personal Technologies.

[10]  F. A. Geldard,et al.  The Cutaneous "Rabbit": A Perceptual Illusion , 1972, Science.

[11]  Abdulmotaleb El-Saddik,et al.  Investigating the influence of temporal intensity changes on apparent movement phenomenon , 2009, 2009 IEEE International Conference on Virtual Environments, Human-Computer Interfaces and Measurements Systems.

[12]  Ali Israr,et al.  Control space of apparent haptic motion , 2011, 2011 IEEE World Haptics Conference.

[13]  Jean M. Henscheid,et al.  The Fundamentals , 2013 .

[14]  M. Sile O'Modhrain,et al.  Cutaneous Grooves: Composing for the Sense of Touch , 2002 .

[15]  I. Summers Tactile Aids for the Hearing Impaired , 1992 .

[16]  G. Gescheider Psychophysics: The Fundamentals , 1997 .

[17]  C. Sherrick A scale for rate of tactual vibration. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  Masaya Hirashima,et al.  The “Cutaneous Rabbit” Hopping out of the Body , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Stephen A. Brewster,et al.  Mobile Multi-actuator Tactile Displays , 2007, HAID.

[20]  Hideyuki Sawada,et al.  Tactile information transmission by apparent movement phenomenon using shape-memory alloy device , 2006 .

[21]  Mark Lehto,et al.  A review of: “Virtual Reality Technology” Grigore Burdea and Philippe Coiffet John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1994 , 1996 .

[22]  Philippe Coiffet,et al.  Virtual Reality Technology , 2003, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[23]  Jaehoon Jung,et al.  Perceived Magnitude and Power Consumption of Vibration Feedback in Mobile Devices , 2007, HCI.