Judging the accuracy of a likelihood judgment: the case of smoking risk
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Tversky,et al. Unpacking, repacking, and anchoring: advances in support theory. , 1997 .
[2] Paul Slovic,et al. Probability, danger, and coercion , 1995 .
[3] W. Viscusi. Comment: The Perils of Qualitative Smoking Risk Measures , 2000 .
[4] Wibecke Brun,et al. Ambiguous probabilities: when doesp=0.3 reflect a possibility, and when does it express a doubt? , 2000 .
[5] G. Wells,et al. The Alternative-Outcomes Effect , 1998 .
[6] Baruch Fischhoff,et al. Fifty–fifty = 50%? , 1999 .
[7] L. Kirkpatrick,et al. Cognitive-experiential self-theory and subjective probability: further evidence for two conceptual systems. , 1992 .
[8] Gerd Gigerenzer,et al. How to Improve Bayesian Reasoning Without Instruction: Frequency Formats , 1995 .
[9] Seymour Epstein,et al. The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. , 1999 .
[10] N. Weinstein. Accuracy of smokers' risk perceptions , 1998, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.
[11] J. Eiser. Risk judgements reflect belief strength, not bias , 1994 .
[12] R F Nease,et al. Perceptions of breast cancer risk and screening effectiveness in women younger than 50 years of age. , 1995, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.
[13] D. Budescu,et al. Processing Linguistic Probabilities: General Principles and Empirical Evidence , 1995 .
[14] Douglas A. Kysar,et al. Taking behavioralism seriously: some evidence of market manipulation. , 1999, Harvard law review.
[15] Windschitl. The Binary Additivity of Subjective Probability Does not Indicate the Binary Complementarity of Perceived Certainty. , 2000, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.
[16] Rami Zwick,et al. Comparing the calibration and coherence of numerical and verbal probability judgments , 1993 .
[17] B. Fischhoff,et al. Fault trees: Sensitivity of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation. , 1978 .
[18] P. Slovic,et al. Violence Risk Assessment and Risk Communication: The Effects of Using Actual Cases, Providing Instruction, and Employing Probability Versus Frequency Formats , 2000, Law and human behavior.
[19] S. Epstein,et al. Conflict Between Intuitive and Rational Processing: When People Behave Against Their Better Judgment , 1994 .
[20] M H Birnbaum,et al. Judgments of proportions. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] Nigel Harvey,et al. Are absolute frequencies, relative frequencies, or both effective in reducing cognitive biases? , 2000 .
[22] Karl Halvor Teigen,et al. Studies in subjective probability III: The unimportance of alternatives , 1983 .
[23] W. Klein,et al. Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[24] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Associative and rule-based processing: A connectionist interpretation of dual-process models. , 1999 .
[25] Gary L. Wells,et al. Measuring psychological uncertainty: Verbal versus numeric methods , 1996 .
[26] P. Slovic. What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescents' perceptions of short‐term and long‐term consequences of smoking , 2000 .
[27] S. Reddy,et al. Measurement Errors in Probability Judgments , 1996 .
[28] Paul Slovic,et al. Rejoinder: the perils of Viscusi's analyses of smoking risk perceptions , 2000 .
[29] William C. Howell,et al. A test of task influences in uncertainty measurement , 1982 .
[30] S. Epstein,et al. The Generality of the Ratio-Bias Phenomenon , 1995 .
[31] R. Buehler,et al. Frequency, Probability, and Prediction: Easy Solutions to Cognitive Illusions? , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[32] Elke U. Weber,et al. The Interpretation of 'Likely' Depends on the Context, But 70% is 70% - Right? The Influence of Associative Processes on Perceived Certainty , 1999 .
[33] S. Sloman. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .
[34] Norbert Schwarz,et al. Response Scales: Effects of Category Range on Reported Behavior and Comparative Judgments , 1985 .
[35] K. Teigen. Subjective sampling distributions and the additivity of estimates , 1974 .
[36] Cheryl Frenck-Mestre,et al. Determinants of numerical versus verbal probabilities , 1993 .
[37] W. C. Howell,et al. Uncertainty measurement: A cognitive taxonomy , 1978 .
[38] R. Borland. What do people's estimates of smoking related risk mean? , 1997 .
[39] G. Gigerenzer. Why the distinction between single-event probabilities and frequencies is important for psychology (and vice versa). , 1994 .
[40] J. Eiser,et al. Accidents, Disease, and the Greenhouse Effect: Effects of Response Categories on Estimates of Risk , 1991 .
[41] A. Tversky,et al. Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability. , 1994 .
[42] Peter Whalley,et al. The Supra-Additivity of Subjective Probability , 1983 .
[43] W. Kip Viscusi,et al. Smoking: Making the Risky Decision , 1992 .
[44] W. Viscusi. Do Smokers Underestimate Risks? , 1990, Journal of Political Economy.
[45] W. Kip Viscusi,et al. Age Variations in Risk Perceptions and Smoking Decisions , 1991 .
[46] Reid Hastie,et al. Revision of beliefs when a hypothesis is eliminated from consideration. , 1985 .
[47] Kimihiko Yamagishi. When a 12.86% Mortality is More Dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for Risk Communication , 1997 .
[48] S. Epstein. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.