Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: differentiating between scale and bias.

Previous investigations on the subjective scale of numerical representations assumed that the scale type can be inferred directly from stimulus-response mapping. This is not a valid assumption, as mapping from the subjective scale into behavior may be nonlinear and/or distorted by response bias. Here we present a method for differentiating between logarithmic and linear hypotheses robust to the effect of distorting processes. The method exploits the idea that a scale is defined by transformational rules and that combinatorial operations with stimulus magnitudes should be closed under admissible transformations on the subjective scale. The method was implemented with novel variants of the number line task. In the line-marking task, participants marked the position of an Arabic numeral within an interval defined by various starting numbers and lengths. In the line construction task, participants constructed an interval given its part. Two alternative approaches to the data analysis, numerical and analytical, were used to evaluate the linear and log components. Our results are consistent with the linear hypothesis about the subjective scale with responses affected by a bias to overestimate small magnitudes and underestimate large magnitudes. We also observed that in the line-marking task, participants tended to overestimate as the interval start increased, and in the line construction task, they tended to overconstruct as the interval length increased. This finding suggests that magnitudes were encoded differently in the 2 tasks: in terms of their absolute magnitudes in the line-marking task and in terms of numerical differences in the line construction task.

[1]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[2]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[3]  L. E. Krueger,et al.  Single judgments of numerosity , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  R. Luce,et al.  On the possible psychophysical laws. , 1959, Psychological review.

[6]  S. S. Stevens Issues in psychophysical measurement. , 1971 .

[7]  Mieko Nakamura,et al.  CENTRAL TENDENCY OF DECISION MAKING UNDER UNCERTAINTY , 1987 .

[8]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  H. Hollingworth The Central Tendency of Judgment , 1910 .

[10]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[11]  I. Spence Visual psychophysics of simple graphical elements. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning , 2005 .

[13]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[14]  L. Hedges,et al.  Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.

[15]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Handbook of experimental psychology , 1951 .

[16]  F. Restle Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .

[17]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  W P Banks,et al.  Two subjective scales of number , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Reading space into numbers – a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect , 2008, Cognition.

[20]  Paul B. Buckley,et al.  Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[22]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Children's early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  H. Helson Adaptation-level as frame of reference for prediction of psychophysical data. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.

[26]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[27]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Mathematical Cognition , 2005 .

[28]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[30]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[31]  S. S. Steivens Measurement, Statistics, and the Schemapiric View , 1968 .

[32]  S. S. Stevens On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.

[33]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .

[34]  Neil Stewart,et al.  Psychophysics and the judgment of price: Judging complex objects on a non-physical dimension elicits sequential effects like those in perceptual tasks , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.

[35]  S. S. Stevens Mathematics, measurement, and psychophysics. , 1951 .

[36]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Multiple spatial representations of number: evidence for co-existing compressive and linear scales , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[38]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures , 2008, Science.

[39]  J. Parkman,et al.  Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[41]  Guy J. Groen,et al.  Temporal aspects of simple addition and comparison , 1971 .

[42]  R. Cheloha,et al.  The of a Development , 2004 .

[43]  M. G. Preston,et al.  An experimental study of the auction-value of an uncertain outcome. , 1948, The American journal of psychology.