Effects of masking noise on laryngeal resistance for breathy, normal, and pressed voice.

PURPOSE The purpose of the present study was to explore the effects of masking noise on laryngeal resistance for breathy, normal, and pressed voice in vocally trained women. METHOD Eighteen vocally trained women produced breathy, normal, and pressed voice across 7 fundamental frequencies during a repeated CV utterance of /pi/ under normal and masked auditory feedback. Dependent variables were mean and standard deviation of laryngeal resistance (LR; cmH2O/l/s). RESULTS LR values for breathy and normal voice remained constant across normal and masked auditory feedback, whereas LR values for pressed voice increased significantly from normal to masked auditory feedback. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that both voice pattern and feedback condition influenced the stability of the LR data. Specifically, the pressed voice pattern may be more susceptible to auditory feedback influence because it was less stable than the breathy and the normal voice patterns. Future investigation should continue to explore the relevance of auditory feedback for theoretical and clinical issues surrounding voice.

[1]  J Sundberg,et al.  Articulatory Fo perturbations and auditory feedback. , 1988, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[2]  G M Siegel,et al.  The influence of selected masking noises on Lombard and sidetone amplification effects. , 1976, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[3]  Johan Sundberg,et al.  Significance of auditory and kinesthetic feedback to singers' pitch control. , 2002, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[4]  I. Titze,et al.  Measurement of vocal fold intraglottal pressure and impact stress. , 1994, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[5]  J. Elman Effects of frequency-shifted feedback on the pitch of vocal productions. , 1981, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  D. Berry,et al.  A quantitative output-cost ratio in voice production. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[7]  P. Alku,et al.  A comparison of glottal voice source quantification parameters in breathy, normal and pressed phonation of female and male speakers. , 1996, Folia phoniatrica et logopaedica : official organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics.

[8]  T. Hixon,et al.  A clinical method for estimating laryngeal airway resistance during vowel production. , 1981, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[9]  Ingo R Titze,et al.  Regulating glottal airflow in phonation: application of the maximum power transfer theorem to a low dimensional phonation model. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  C. Larson,et al.  Voice F0 responses to pitch-shifted auditory feedback: a preliminary study. , 1997, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[11]  Jay J Bauer,et al.  Vocal responses to unanticipated perturbations in voice loudness feedback: an automatic mechanism for stabilizing voice amplitude. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  J. Sundberg,et al.  Spectral correlates of glottal voice source waveform characteristics. , 1989, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[13]  L. Glaze,et al.  Preliminary study of two methods of treatment for laryngeal nodules. , 1995, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[14]  R. Daniloff,et al.  A Reply to Coleman and Schliesser’s “Comment on Articulation and Stress/Juncture Production Under Oral Anesthetization and Masking” , 1972 .

[15]  Lois L. Elliott,et al.  The Role of Hearing in Controlling Voice Fundamental Frequency , 1970 .

[16]  C. Larson,et al.  Voice F0 responses to pitch-shifted voice feedback during English speech. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  F Klingholz,et al.  Computer aided evaluation of phonetograms. , 1992, Computer methods and programs in biomedicine.

[18]  Elizabeth U. Grillo,et al.  Laryngeal resistance distinguished pressed, normal, and breathy voice in vocally untrained females , 2009, Logopedics, phoniatrics, vocology.

[19]  Elizabeth U. Grillo Clinical investigation of the Global Voice Therapy Model , 2012, International journal of speech-language pathology.

[20]  Ciara Leydon,et al.  The role of auditory feedback in sustaining vocal vibrato. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  H. Lane,et al.  The Lombard Sign and the Role of Hearing in Speech , 1971 .

[22]  C. Larson,et al.  Effects of simultaneous perturbations of voice pitch and loudness feedback on voice F0 and amplitude control. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  C. Larson,et al.  Voice F0 responses to manipulations in pitch feedback. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  I R Titze,et al.  The effect of subglottal resonance upon vocal fold vibration. , 1997, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[25]  Ronald J. Baken,et al.  Clinical measurement of speech and voice , 1987 .

[26]  Elizabeth U. Grillo,et al.  Evidence for distinguishing pressed, normal, resonant, and breathy voice qualities by laryngeal resistance and vocal efficiency in vocally trained subjects. , 2008, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[27]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of task instructions and oscillation frequency on bimanual coordination , 1996, Psychological research.

[28]  J Sundberg,et al.  A STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF SUBGLOTTAL PRESSURE , FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCY AND MODE OF PHONATION ON THE VOICE SOURCE , 2007 .

[29]  Carole T Ferrand Relationship between masking levels and phonatory stability in normal-speaking women. , 2006, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[30]  Johan Sundberg,et al.  Effects of a professional solo singer education on auditory and kinesthetic feedback--a longitudinal study of singers' pitch control. , 2004, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[31]  H. S. Osborne,et al.  The international electrotechnical commission , 1953, Electrical Engineering.

[32]  Yi Xu,et al.  Compensation for pitch-shifted auditory feedback during the production of Mandarin tone sequences. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  W. D. Ward,et al.  Pitch performance in singing without auditory feedback , 1975 .

[34]  John F. Houde,et al.  Compensatory responses to brief perturbations of speech amplitude , 2005 .

[35]  C. Sapienza,et al.  Three Treatments for Teachers With Voice Disorders , 2003 .

[36]  I R Titze,et al.  Vocal intensity in speakers and singers. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[38]  H. Hoffman,et al.  Comparison of Aerodynamic and Electroglottographic Parameters in Evaluating Clinically Relevant Voicing Patterns , 1994, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[39]  J. Perkell,et al.  Glottal airflow and transglottal air pressure measurements for male and female speakers in soft, normal, and loud voice. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[40]  I. Titze Vocal fold physiology : frontiers in basic science , 1993 .

[41]  M Rothenberg,et al.  Measurement of airflow in speech. , 1977, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[42]  Jay J Bauer,et al.  Voice responses to changes in pitch of voice or tone auditory feedback. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.