Vection underwater illustrates the limitations of neutral buoyancy as a microgravity analog
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Harris,et al. Long-duration head down bed rest as an analog of microgravity: Effects on the static perception of upright , 2021, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[2] Athanasios Alexopoulos,et al. Mechanisms. , 2021, Deutsches Arzteblatt international.
[3] Jonathan W. Kelly,et al. The effect of water immersion on vection in virtual reality , 2021, Scientific Reports.
[4] L. Harris,et al. When gravity is not where it should be: How perceived orientation affects visual self-motion processing. , 2021, PloS one.
[5] O. White,et al. The gravitational imprint on sensorimotor planning and control. , 2020, Journal of neurophysiology.
[6] A. Bukley,et al. Horizontal and Vertical Distance Perception in Altered Gravity , 2020, Scientific Reports.
[7] Zelie Britton,et al. Vestibular and Multi-Sensory Influences Upon Self-Motion Perception and the Consequences for Human Behavior , 2019, Front. Neurol..
[8] Christopher K. Rhea,et al. Changes in Posture Following a Single Session of Long-Duration Water Immersion. , 2018, Journal of applied biomechanics.
[9] John B Charles,et al. Practicing for space underwater: inventing neutral buoyancy training, 1963-1968. , 2015, Endeavour.
[10] Bernhard E Riecke,et al. Can walking motions improve visually induced rotational self-motion illusions in virtual reality? , 2015, Journal of vision.
[11] Scott J. Wood,et al. Rocking or Rolling – Perception of Ambiguous Motion after Returning from Space , 2014, PloS one.
[12] Laurence R. Harris,et al. How Much Gravity Is Needed to Establish the Perceptual Upright? , 2014, PloS one.
[13] Jennifer L. Campos,et al. Contributions of visual and proprioceptive information to travelled distance estimation during changing sensory congruencies , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] Gilles Clément,et al. Distance and Size Perception in Astronauts during Long-Duration Spaceflight , 2013, Life.
[15] Stephen Palmisano,et al. Vection in Depth during Treadmill Walking , 2013, Perception.
[16] A. Berthoz,et al. Weightlessness alters up/down asymmetries in the perception of self-motion , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] O. Bock,et al. Changed joint position sense and muscle activity in simulated weightlessness by water immersion. , 2013, Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine.
[18] Pearl S. Guterman,et al. Perception of smooth and perturbed vection in short-duration microgravity , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] O. Blanke,et al. The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans , 2011, Brain Research Reviews.
[20] Christine J. Ziemer,et al. Estimating distance in real and virtual environments: Does order make a difference? , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[21] D. Angelaki,et al. Basic and Clinical Aspects of Vertigo and Dizziness How Vestibular Neurons Solve the Tilt/translation Ambiguity Comparison of Brainstem, Cerebellum, and Thalamus , 2022 .
[22] M. Reschke,et al. Neuroscience in Space , 2009, American Journal of Neuroradiology.
[23] Kenneth H. Britten,et al. Mechanisms of self-motion perception. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.
[24] David Waller,et al. Interaction With an Immersive Virtual Environment Corrects Users' Distance Estimates , 2007, Hum. Factors.
[25] Laurence R. Harris,et al. Travel distance estimation from visual motion by leaky path integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[26] R. Bondar,et al. The Neurolab Spacelab Mission: Neuroscience Research in Space: Results From the STS-90, Neurolab Spacelab Mission , 2005 .
[27] J. T. Richards,et al. Exposure to a rotating virtual environment during treadmill locomotion causes adaptation in heading direction , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] Markus Lappe,et al. Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.
[29] J E Zacher,et al. The relative role of visual and non-visual cues in determining the perceived direction of "up": experiments in parabolic flight. , 2005, Acta astronautica.
[30] Peter Willemsen,et al. The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Indoor Environments , 2005, Perception.
[31] D. Angelaki,et al. Gravity or translation: central processing of vestibular signals to detect motion or tilt. , 2003, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[32] Helen E. Ross,et al. Why do objects appear enlarged under water , 2003 .
[33] B. J. Yates,et al. Convergence of limb, visceral, and vertical semicircular canal or otolith inputs onto vestibular nucleus neurons , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[34] F. Mast,et al. The effect of water immersion on postural and visual orientation. , 1999, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[35] A M Bronstein,et al. The Interaction of Otolith and Proprioceptive Information in the Perception of Verticality: The Effects of Labyrinthine and CNS Disease , 1999, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[36] Thomas Rosemeier,et al. Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and visual signals for postural control and motion perception under terrestrial and microgravity conditions—a conceptual model , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.
[37] C M Oman,et al. Horizontal angular VOR changes in orbital and parabolic flight: human neurovestibular studies on SLS-2. , 1996, Journal of applied physiology.
[38] L R Young,et al. Tactile influences on astronaut visual spatial orientation: human neurovestibular studies on SLS-2. , 1996, Journal of applied physiology.
[39] Roland Maurer,et al. What is modelling for? a critical review of the models of path integration , 1995 .
[40] D L Harm,et al. Perceived self-orientation and self-motion in microgravity, after landing and during preflight adaptation training. , 1993, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[41] Chizu Kano,et al. The Perception of Self-Motion Induced by Peripheral Visual Information in Sitting and Supine Postures , 1991 .
[42] L R Young,et al. Microgravity enhances the relative contribution of visually-induced motion sensation. , 1990, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[43] L R Young,et al. Perception of linear acceleration in weightlessness. , 1990, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[44] Walter C. Gogel,et al. A two-process theory of the response to size and distance , 1987, Perception & Psychophysics.
[45] M F Reschke,et al. Otolith tilt-translation reinterpretation following prolonged weightlessness: implications for preflight training. , 1985, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[46] L R Young,et al. Spatial orientation in weightlessness and readaptation to earth's gravity. , 1984, Science.
[47] L. R. Young,et al. M.I.T./Canadian vestibular experiments on the Spacelab-1 mission: 1. Sensory adaptation to weightlessness and readaptation to one-g: an overview , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.
[48] V. Henn,et al. The velocity response of vestibular nucleus neurons during vestibular, visual, and combined angular acceleration , 1979, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] T. Brandt,et al. Arthrokinetic nystagmus and ego-motion sensation , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.
[50] N. Wade. The effect of water immersion on perception of the visual vertical. , 1973, British Journal of Psychology.
[51] O F Trout,et al. Water Immersion Reduced-Gravity Simulation , 1969, Human factors.
[52] O. F. Trout,et al. Water immersion simulation of extravehicular activities by astronauts. , 1967 .
[53] M. M. Taylor,et al. PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions , 1967 .
[54] W. Epstein,et al. The current status of the size-distance hypotheses. , 1961, Psychological bulletin.
[55] E E BARNARD,et al. Visual Problems under Water , 1961, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.
[56] Mary Van Baalen,et al. Tracking Historical NASA EVA Training: Lifetime Surveillance of Astronaut Health (LSAH) Development of the EVA Suit Exposure Tracker (EVA SET) , 2017 .
[57] G. DeAngelis,et al. Distributed Visual–Vestibular Processing in the Cerebral Cortex of Man and Macaque , 2017 .
[58] Laurence R. Young,et al. Perception of the Body in Space: Mechanisms , 2011 .
[59] U. Büttner,et al. Vestibular nuclei activity and eye movements in the alert monkey during sinusoidal optokinetic stimulation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[60] K. E. Money,et al. Visually-induced tilt during parabolic flights , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[61] Daniel M Merfeld,et al. Rotation otolith tilt-translation reinterpretation (ROTTR) hypothesis: a new hypothesis to explain neurovestibular spaceflight adaptation. , 2003, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[62] James E. Zacher,et al. The Role of Visual Cues in Microgravity Spatial Orientation , 2003 .
[63] Michael Jenkin,et al. Humans can use optic flow to estimate distance of travel , 2001, Vision Research.
[64] W. Norfleet,et al. Immersed false vertical room. A new motion sickness model. , 1998, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[65] J. Dichgans,et al. Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .
[66] Horst Mittelstaedt,et al. Mechanismen der Orientierung ohne richtende Außenreize , 1973 .
[67] H E Ross,et al. Orientation to the vertical in free divers. , 1969, Aerospace medicine.