Distinct neural mechanisms for repetition effects of visual objects

Repetition of visually common objects was examined in relation to prior intentional learning and memory status using a delayed match-to-sample task in humans. Both response time and two temporally separate event-related potential (ERP) components indexed repetition. The early repetition effect ( approximately 200-550 ms) evoked more ERP responses for repeated visual objects, and was diminished by prior intentional learning (old/new) or being maintained in working memory (targets/distracters). In contrast, the late repetition effect (after approximately 550 ms) evoked reduced ERP activation for repeated items, and was not affected by prior learning or working memory status. Our source localization results indicate that the late and posterior repetition effect in visual cortex is consistent with repetition suppression results reported in monkey physiology and human fMRI studies. Meanwhile, the early and anterior repetition effect, in temporal pole and frontal cortices, is modulated by explicit memory mechanisms.

[1]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Ken A. Paller,et al.  Neural Manifestations of Memory with and without Awareness , 2003, Neuron.

[3]  G. Orban,et al.  Selectivity of Neuronal Adaptation Does Not Match Response Selectivity: A Single-Cell Study of the fMRI Adaptation Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[4]  K. Paller Neurocognitive foundations of human memory , 2000 .

[5]  G. Boynton,et al.  Adaptation: from single cells to BOLD signals , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  M. D Rugg,et al.  The effect of repetition lag on electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of visual object priming , 2004, NeuroImage.

[7]  Alex Martin,et al.  Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  T. Penney,et al.  Repetition related ERP effects in a visual object target detection task. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[10]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Oscillatory brain activity dissociates between associative stimulus content in a repetition priming task in the human EEG. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Complementary neural mechanisms for tracking items in human working memory. , 2000, Science.

[12]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[13]  A. Wagner,et al.  Working Memory Contributions to Human Learning and Remembering , 1999, Neuron.

[14]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Differential effects of normal aging on sources of standard N1, target N1 and target P300 auditory event-related brain potentials revealed by low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA). , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[15]  M. Rugg Event-related brain potentials dissociate repetition effects of high-and low-frequency words , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Alex Martin,et al.  Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Dissociation of Semantic Priming, Word and Non-Word Repetition Effects by Event-Related Potentials , 1987 .

[19]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Selective Attention Modulates Neural Substrates of Repetition Priming and Implicit Visual Memory: Suppressions and Enhancements Revealed by fMRI , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Lars Nyberg,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of aging : linking cognitive and cerebral aging , 2004 .

[21]  Ava J. Senkfor,et al.  Memory for words and novel visual patterns: repetition, recognition, and encoding effects in the event-related brain potential. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Yang Jiang,et al.  Age effects on brain activity during repetition priming of targets and distracters , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  M. Chun,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Learning-Related Repetition Attenuation Effects in Human Parahippocampal Cortex , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The rise and fall of priming: how visual exposure shapes cortical representations of objects. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  M. Rugg,et al.  Modulation of event-related potentials by word repetition: the effects of inter-item lag. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[26]  R. Henson Neuroimaging studies of priming , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  Ching-Yune C. Sylvester,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory and Aging , 2005 .

[28]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Object-selective cortex exhibits performance-independent repetition suppression. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Michael C. Doyle,et al.  Modulation of event-related potentials by the repetition of drawings of novel objects. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[30]  R. Henson,et al.  Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition and priming. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  H. Begleiter,et al.  Event related potentials during object recognition tasks , 1995, Brain Research Bulletin.

[32]  B. Renault,et al.  Face and shape repetition effects in humans: a spatio‐temporal ERP study , 1997, Neuroreport.

[33]  D Friedman,et al.  Event‐related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[34]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  The effects of immediate stimulus repetition on reaction time and event-related potentials in tasks of different complexity , 1994 .

[35]  G. Boynton,et al.  Orientation-Specific Adaptation in Human Visual Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Repetition suppression of faces is modulated by emotion. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  T. Shallice,et al.  Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. , 2000, Science.

[38]  M. Rugg,et al.  Lexical contribution to nonword-repetition effects: Evidence from event-related potentials , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[39]  Sophie Dupont,et al.  Investigating temporal pole function by functional imaging. , 2002, Epileptic disorders : international epilepsy journal with videotape.

[40]  M. Kutas,et al.  Overt and covert identification of fragmented objects inferred from performance and electrophysiological measures. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  D. Schacter,et al.  Interactions Between Forms of Memory: When Priming Hinders New Episodic Learning , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal , 2001, Nature.

[43]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Aging and Repetition Priming for Targets and Distracters in a Working Memory Task , 2006, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[44]  Chunyan Guo,et al.  Brain potentials distinguish new and studied objects during working memory , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[45]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Event-related brain potentials. , 1990 .

[46]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[47]  E. Halgren,et al.  Dynamic Statistical Parametric Mapping Combining fMRI and MEG for High-Resolution Imaging of Cortical Activity , 2000, Neuron.

[48]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[49]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  Christian J Fiebach,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms of Repetition Priming in Occipitotemporal Cortex: Spatiotemporal Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Electroencephalography , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Roberto D. Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) : review, new comparisons, and new validation , 2002 .

[52]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.

[53]  Stefan R Schweinberger,et al.  N200, N250r, and N400 event-related brain potentials reveal three loci of repetition priming for familiar names. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[54]  K. Paller,et al.  Distinguishing source memory and item memory: Brain potentials at encoding and retrieval , 2006, Brain Research.

[55]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[56]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Effects of attention and emotion on repetition priming and their modulation by cholinergic enhancement. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.