Genetic and environmental contributions to the covariance between occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ: A study of twins

Scores of occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ were obtained for 507 monozygotic and 575 dizygotic male twin pairs born 1931–1935 and 1944–1960. A multivariate genetic analysis with statistics from different cohorts showed heterogeneity between cohorts, and analyses were performed in four separate cohorts. The only set of results which departed clearly from the rest was found for the group born 1931–1935, where the ratio of environmental to genetic effects exceeded those of the other groups. Typical heritability values in the three youngest groups (weighted means) were .43, .51, and .66 for occupation, education, and IQ, respectively. The values in the oldest group were .16, .10, and .37, but this sample is small and the estimates are unstable. Genetic variance influencing educational attainment also contributed approximately onefourth of the genetic variance for occupational status and nearly half the genetic variance for IQ. The values for the between-families variances (reflecting family environment and assortative mating) varied from 2 to 35% in the three youngest groups but were higher for education (62%) and IQ (45%) in the oldest groups. All the between-families variance was common to all three variables. For educational attainment and IQ, the bulk of this between-families variance is probably genetic variance due to assortative mating. The common-factor environmental within-family variances were generally small, and the specific estimates seemed to contain mainly measurement error.

[1]  J. Sundet,et al.  Heritability analysis of the WAIS subtests: A study of twins. , 1984 .

[2]  T. Bouchard,et al.  Familial studies of intelligence: a review. , 1981, Science.

[3]  D. Fulker Multivariate extensions of a biometrical model of twin data. , 1978, Progress in clinical and biological research.

[4]  M. Ng Genetics of sexual and social attitudes in twins. , 1978 .

[5]  L. Eaves,et al.  Education policy and the heritability of educational attainment , 2016, Nature.

[6]  D. Fulker,et al.  Familial resemblance for educational attainment , 1983, Behavior genetics.

[7]  L J Eaves,et al.  The genetical analysis of covariance structure , 1977, Heredity.

[8]  N. Martin,et al.  Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour , 1978, Heredity.

[9]  S. R. Wilson,et al.  Bias in the estimation of heritability from truncated samples of twins , 1982, Behavior genetics.

[10]  N. Martin,et al.  A note on the generalized effects of assortative mating , 1984, Behavior genetics.

[11]  N. Martin Genetics of sexual and social attitudes in twins. , 1978, Progress in clinical and biological research.

[12]  A. Tellegen,et al.  Recruitment bias in twin research: The rule of two-thirds reconsidered , 1987, Behavior genetics.

[13]  J. Hamill,et al.  Somatic hybridisation using a double mutant of nicotiana tabacum , 1984, Heredity.

[14]  P. Taubman Kinometrics : determinants of socioeconomic success within and between families , 1978 .

[15]  No recruitment bias for questionnaire data related to IQ in classical twin studies , 1989 .

[16]  A. Heath,et al.  No decline in assortative mating for educational level , 1985, Behavior genetics.

[17]  P. Magnus,et al.  Predicting zygosity in Norwegian twin pairs born 1915–1960 , 1983, Clinical genetics.

[18]  Multivariate extensions of a biometrical model of twin data. , 1978 .

[19]  J. Sundet,et al.  Genetic and environmental contributions to the covariation between the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) subtests: A study of twins , 1986, Behavior genetics.

[20]  N. Martin,et al.  Is there only one set of genes for different abilities? A reanalysis of the National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test (NMSQT) data , 1984, Behavior genetics.

[21]  Per Magnus,et al.  On the Question of Secular Trends in the Heritability of Intelligence Test Scores: A Study of Norwegian Twins , 1988 .